- Review
- Open Access
- Published:
The effects of Beta-Endorphin: state change modification
Fluids and Barriers of the CNS volume 12, Article number: 3 (2015)
Abstract
Beta-endorphin (β-END) is an opioid neuropeptide which has an important role in the development of hypotheses concerning the non-synaptic or paracrine communication of brain messages. This kind of communication between neurons has been designated volume transmission (VT) to differentiate it clearly from synaptic communication. VT occurs over short as well as long distances via the extracellular space in the brain, as well as via the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) flowing through the ventricular spaces inside the brain and the arachnoid space surrounding the central nervous system (CNS). To understand how β-END can have specific behavioral effects, we use the notion behavioral state, inspired by the concept of machine state, coming from Turing (Proc London Math Soc, Series 2,42:230-265, 1937). In section 1.4 the sequential organization of male rat behavior is explained showing that an animal is not free to switch into another state at any given moment. Funneling-constraints restrict the number of possible behavioral transitions in specific phases while at other moments in the sequence the transition to other behavioral states is almost completely open. The effects of β-END on behaviors like food intake and sexual behavior, and the mechanisms involved in reward, meditation and pain control are discussed in detail. The effects on the sequential organization of behavior and on state transitions dominate the description of these effects.
Introduction
States
An organism, from a single cell to Homo sapiens, continuously responds to input from its environment with (if all is well) an adequate action. Such input-action pairs only partially describe the organism. Indeed, the same external stimuli may give rise to a different reaction. In that case we say that the organism is in a different (internal) state. In this way states are defined as action tendencies. The state of an organism may vary from moment to moment. Therefore we define the state of an organism (the notion also applies to mechanical systems with sensors and actuators) at a given moment t, as the way it acts when receiving certain stimuli at moment t. Thus a state is an idealized mathematical notion, that usually cannot be known in full, but that is nevertheless very useful: it can be approximated and can serve for theoretical considerations. The notion of state is used in computer science [1] and also extensively in mathematical system theory. Prior to this, statistical mechanics described the state of a gas in a vat as elements of a space of dimension 6.10^23, where the 6 stands for the 3 position coordinates plus 3 velocity components of the 10^23 (Avogadro’s number) molecules in a given volume. Such complex states cannot be determined empirically nor theoretically, but serve to derive the well-established laws of thermodynamics.
Approximating states, sub-states
As approximation to a state in an organism, one can consider a vector of variables having a given value [2]. Indeed, in the ideal case of having all possible values, such a vector completely determines the behavior of the organism, depending on presented stimuli. A sub-state is a part of this vector of values, determining only partly the input-action relation. For the notion of state one may restrict oneself to relevant forms of input and action, depending on the context of the subject matter. These are sub-states relevant in a certain scientific context. For example, in studying a species one may restrict oneself to feeding or to sexual behavior. In such cases one speaks about behavioral states. For Homo sapiens another restriction is useful: it makes sense to focus on signals available to consciousness and to intended actions. Then one speaks about mental states.
The notion of state is a so-called higher-order mathematical notion. A state is not involved with just one input signal and one action signal, but with a whole class of input-action signals. Making restriction eases the fact that states are higher-order mechanisms and can be helpful to obtain experimental or theoretical results. The well-known human emotions (like anger, fear, desire, surprise, disgust) are all examples of mental sub-states of a more general mental state. They are sub-states because one can be angry and fight, or angry and submissive, hence indeed some parameters are lacking.
Phases: keeping some sub-states fixed
By the definition above, a state is momentary, occurring at a precise moment in time. A sequence of related (e.g. when a certain sub-state being fixed) states during an uninterrupted interval of time can be called a phase. An example is an animal in the state of being hungry. It is looking for food in order to eat. Usually the state of hunger remains, even if food is found and the animal is eating. In this phase of eating, the sub-state hunger is more or less fixed. It has to persist for obvious reasons: one bite of food is not enough. But the persistence of hunger is not exact; it (gradually) diminishes, for otherwise the animal would (in the condition of abundance of food) never stop eating. Another example of a phase with a constant sub-state is sleep. The mechanisms for sensorial input and muscular output are for a large part blocked. But during the phase of sleeping not all sub-states are the same: one can distinguish rapid-eye movement (REM) and non-REM sleep. In both examples phases may be divided into several sub-phases.
It should be emphasized that the notions of state, sub-state, phase, and sub-phase are all quite natural and familiar. Talking about the weather, the notion of (momentary) state corresponds to the familiar values indicating the temperature, air-pressure, direction and speed of the wind, humidity, etcetera, all at one given moment. The notion of phase that lasts for a certain time interval applies to what we usually call the weather. Indeed, a rain storm consists of an uninterrupted flow of states having in common that it is both raining and windy. Exactly how much rain is falling and whether there is also lightning (a sub-state at a given moment) may vary during the interval of the phase of the rainstorm. So there is place for sub-phases: “During the rainstorm the lightning lasted unusually long.” We see that the notion of (momentary) state is fundamental, while phase refers to a continuous time interval during which the momentary states are similar but not necessarily equal, for example by keeping some of the sub-states constant. The concept of phase is very different from that of state. There is a precise mathematical definition of what is a state. What is a phase depends on what one considers to be comparable. When exactly does a storm end? One could say: “If no longer there are gusts of wind with speed 150 km/hour.” Here there is a place for choice (possibly very relevant for aeronautics). In the case of state at a given moment, there is no choice.
Change and maintenance of states
In an organism with a CNS, induction of a new sub-state in which a couple of parameters are to be changed, can be organized efficiently using action potentials and synaptic transmission. If, on the other hand, there is a need for maintenance of a sub-state, in the terminology above for a certain phase, there seem to be several natural possibilities for doing this. 1. Sustained neural activity, using synaptic transmission. 2. Local volume transmission (VT) to keep certain chemical parameters locally at the right level. 3. If parameters need to be changed globally into a certain direction, then volume transmission through the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF-VT) is an option (see Table 1). For animals with a developed CNS, we have argued that there is a mixed usage, in particular for neuropeptides like oxytocin and β-END [3–5]. It has been shown that the mechanisms work in parallel: a fast ephemeral axonal message transports the signal of a peptide to relevant areas; after that the slower but longer lasting volume transmission does its work. Via second messengers, the effects of VT may last much longer, up to months, years or even life-long [6–11]. This may be related to the question why there are so many neuropeptides and what is their relation to the variety of different behavioral states.
Numerous findings in the literature can be explained by simply assuming that neuroactive substances, released at specific points in the brain along the ventricular system, reach their distant target areas by ‘going with the flow’ of the CSF. This particular kind of long-distance-VT has been included in all discussions concerning VT [6, 11–15] and in fact β-END was among the very first substances mentioned in relation to VT [9, 11]. Meanwhile, the effects of substances released from particular parts of the brain into the CSF to target distant brain areas have been studied for a variety of substances such as vasopressin, corticotropin-releasing-factor, gonadotropin-releasing-hormone, melatonin and oxytocin [3, 4, 16–22]. Recently, we have reviewed the available evidence showing that β-END is another neuropeptide that can be released into the CSF to affect distant brain areas [5]. Most neurons producing β-END are located in the arcuate nucleus (ARH) of the basal hypothalamus, alongside the third ventricle, but an additional, smaller group has been observed in the caudal brainstem. The involved ARH-neurons produce a mixture of neuropeptides and are known as proopiomelanocortin- (POMC-) neurons (for further details, see [5]).
The present review discusses more specifically the potential of CSF-flow to influence a number of brain areas together, to induce behavioral (state-) changes. The behavioral data, provided below, show on the one hand that certain brain manipulations with β-END have a special effect on specific behavioral transitions, thereby effectively blocking transitions to another behavioral phase. On the other hand, β-END induces general behavioral effects, described by several authors as ‘a state of well-being’ (see below), which also clearly suggests that the effects of this neuropeptide are state-related. The present review focuses on such behavioral effects of β-END and in addition we discuss states and their selection and preservation on the basis of the Turing-model [23] inspired by Turing [1]. Actually it is a hybrid model that is also inspired by the neural nets introduced in Turing (1948), see [24, 25] describing artificial neural nets, a connection already pointed out previously [8].
The sequential nature of behavioral states in human cognition
In Zylberberg et al. [26] and Barendregt & Raffone [23] human cognition has been independently described as a 'discrete hybrid Turing machine’. This means the following: 1. [Discreteness] actions proceed in a serial way (one after the other, like a fast ticking clock, not continuously); 2. [Turing machine] these actions do not only depend on the stimulus (a demonstrable drawback of behaviorism), but also on the state of the organism/machine; 3. [Hybrid machine] how a given stimulus and state produce an action is determined by a parallel neural net (unlike in the classical Turing machine, where these transitions are described by an explicit table). The model is quite simple: important events, state changes and actions, are occurring in a discrete serial fashion, depending on the previous state and the input. Among the possible actions, focusing attention is an important one. This is the full model description. The discreteness of human cognition is supported by psychophysical evidence on visual illusions and periodicities in reaction time (for a review see [27], as well as by electroencephalographic evidence about discrete brain microstates, (see [28]. Paper [27] emphasizes different aspects than does [28]. For example the first paper beautifully answers the question of John von Neumann how it is possible that human cognition answers questions with high accuracy while there is biological noise. The answer by Zylberberg et al. [26] is convincing: “by discretization” (like a CD avoids the noise of an old-fashioned vinyl record). The second paper [28] emphasizes the use of states, and employs the notion of a universal Turing machine (programmable computer) in which states can be used as input. This enables modification of automatic behavior.
The sequential organization of animal behavior: a ‘funnel model’
In a behavioral study of rats [29, 30] the structure of feeding, sexual and agonistic behavior was analyzed by means of an extensive transition analysis of the successive behavioral elements. In some experimental situations, regular transitions were interrupted by electrical stimulation of the ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus of the freely moving rat. This approach showed not only the normal succession of behavioral elements in each of the behavioral sequences, but also the interruptive effects of the medial hypothalamic stimulation. These effects were strongly dependent on the moment of delivery during the behavioral sequence: during the ‘scanning’- or initial phase the normally succeeding transitions towards the appetitive and consummator phases appeared to be completely blocked. When stimulation started during the succeeding, appetitive, phase, the experimental animal tried to ‘switch back’ to the initial phase, as long as possible. However, when the brain stimulation was delivered in the final phase, animals tried to ‘complete the sequence’ before returning into the initial phase. These findings together are the basis for the ‘Funnel Model’, [29–31] describing the organization of behavior of the male rat, as depicted in Figure 1.
The Funnel-Model of the sequential organization of the male rat behavior. The model is based on a transition analysis of the behavioral elements of the male rat occurring during a series of experiments, which included feeding behavior, sexual behavior and territorial aggression, combined with the effects of electrical stimulation of the ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus (VMH). Explanation: see text. We have coined this model the ‘Funnel-Model’, because it illustrates clearly that in phase 1 of the behavioral sequence the animal is relatively free to make choices leading to any possible ‘consummatory act’, or in a wider view, to any behavioral state. Phase 1 can be characterized as a transitional situation, from where any behavioral sequence leading to a specific consummatory act can be performed or from where any possible behavioral state can be reached. At the end of phase 2 the situation is completely different: the male is ‘bound to’ perform the consummatory act, (mostly consisting of a series of physiological reflexes) and the opportunity to select other behavioral transitions is temporarily blocked. Only after completing the consummatory behavior, the ‘freedom’ of phase 1 is available again. The ‘Funnel-model’ illustrates on the one hand that physiological and brain mechanisms are working to support behavioral perseverance and to keep behavior directed to a specific goal, while on the other hand, especially in phase 1, the opportunity is raised to choose another strategy, or to pursue another goal or to reach another state.
In phase 1 the resident animal is just scanning the environment, with some sniffing and locomotion, and for the observer it is not yet clear in which direction behavior will develop. If, however, palatable food or an estrous female or a male intruder is introduced into the resident’s cage, its behavior rapidly changes into the appetitive/procurement phase, with characteristic and species-specific and goal-directed behavioral elements to obtain the food/prey, to perform (pre)copulatory activities or to approach and threaten the male intruder. In the final consummatory/executive phase, food consumption, or ejaculation or biting and fighting occur, until the intruder shows submission or leaves the field. In this final phase, the behavior contains many ‘reflexes’, organized at the level of the brainstem and spinal cord, like chewing and swallowing, ejaculation as controlled by the spinal ejaculation center [31–34] or an extremely fast series of biting and fighting movements. At the end of the consummatory phase the animal may return to the initial phase, when (temporarily) satiated by food or ejaculatory activities, but it may also enter the second phase immediately again if satiation (= ‘negative feedback’-signals) did not occur sufficiently.
In Figure 1 the arrows indicate the number of transitions from and to the successive behavioral phases. At the left side, it is shown that in the early initiation-phase behavioral transitions occur frequently (70%) towards other behavioral sequences. Early in phase 2 a considerable number of transitions (about 40%) still occurs in a direction not leading to the expected consummatory phase, but later in the appetitive phase such an exit-choice is made in less than 10% of the transitions. For that reason we have coined the term Funnel-Model to describe the sequential organization of behavior, because it suggests that the possibility to choose for another behavioral goal becomes more and more restricted. In the laboratory situations studied, 50% of the animals, entering Phase 2, continued towards the appropriate consummatory elements, apparently after some point of no return. The animal had no choice other than completing the sequence and only after completing the total sequence, the animal could either repeat a part of Phase 3 (15%), or enter Phase 2 again (35%) or, under our experimental conditions, enter Phase 1 again (50%). We consider an animal behaving in Phase 1 as least constrained, meaning that all behavioral options are open depending on the internal state and the external stimuli, and that an animal at the very end of Phase 2 is most strongly constrained, and virtually bound to finish the complete sequence by performing the mostly reflexive acts composing the consummatory phase 3.
This Funnel-concept was strongly supported by the, mostly disturbing, effects of electrical stimulation of the ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus (VMH). During the 30-sec stimulation periods, animals did not or hardly entered phase 2. When, however, stimulation happened to start at the very end of Phase 2, animals seemed to finish the sequence as quickly as possible. During VMH-stimulation at the end of the sequence, animals never returned to phases 2 or 3, but immediately entered a vigilant version of Phase 1, remaining there during the 30-second intermittent stimulation period [29–31]. The Funnel-shape of the behavioral progression during a sequence plays an important role in a wider context of animal behavior: coping mechanisms and behavioral states. Considering feeding, sexual behavior and agonistic activities as just three of a variety of possible behavioral sequences, we wish to address two aspects specifically: on the one hand, goal-directed behavior asks for perseverance for a specific goal, and the performing animal should not be diverted by irrelevant external or internal factors for a while. Behavioral funneling supports the animal to stay in a given behavioral sequence or behavioral state, to stay on track until the goal is reached. On the other hand, less constrained periods, like Phase 1 in between specific behavioral sequences, are necessary as switching points in order to allow the animal to choose another behavioral strategy, for entering other states and/or to pursue other goals. The funnel-shape reflects these opposing characteristics in a continuous flow of behavioral performances. β-END appears to be involved in this behavioral flow and the switching points (see sections 3.1 and especially 3.2).
Towards a general role of β-endorphin
Over the last decades, numerous reviews have appeared on the behavioral and physiological aspects of β-END [35–63]. From these, it becomes clear that β-END is involved in a wide variety of functions ranging from the cellular to the behavioral level. Many reviewers discussed the placebo effects possibly occurring on the administration of β-END and other substances influencing the μ-receptor [64–70]. Several of the mentioned reviewers of the effects of β-END proposed unifying concepts to embrace this variety into a general behavioral function, but their proposals seem to be pointing in different directions.
In 1982 Henry assumed that all general effects, for example those affecting the pain-regulating systems in the spinal cord, were induced by circulating opioids [71]. The paper did not yet consider the possibility of β-END messages via the CSF. Henry asked, however, special attention for the possibility that “the activation of a number of functions together may be due to a global activation of opiate receptors throughout the CNS”, which is consistent with the main thesis of our present paper. In the summary (p 239), after comparing the effects of stress, and sexual activities with vigorous dancing and states of trance, Henry concludes that mild activation of the β-END system induces a state of well-being, while stronger activation results in analgesia and euphoria. On the other hand, “when the endorphin system is hypoactive, ……, an increased drive ensues to satisfy a deprived state, whether this is an appetite for food, water, social contact, or sexual satisfaction, etc.” [71]. This paper contained the clear hypothesis that the effects of endorphin depend on the activity state of the endorphin system. In 1984, Akil et al. concluded that “The multiplicity we behold in studying endogenous opioid function is dizzying”, [72], focusing in their review mainly on stress, analgesia and cardiovascular control mechanisms. In 1985 the role of β-END in learning and memory processes was addressed [73–75]. Izquierdo and Netto showed that “ a variety of behavioral experiences activate the β-END system, apparently as a result of novelty; that this activation is mediated by the septo-hippocampo-subicular system; and that this seems to play a role in regulation of the retrieval of learned behavior …….” [74]. Due to its rather long recovery time, the arousal of the β-END system “must be reserved for events that are particularly striking to the animal”. “β-END may obviously play a very important role in adaptive behavior” as well as in developing (alternative) coping strategies (ibidem).
In 1986 the μ-receptor system was discussed [59]. After referring to a variety of behavioral and physiological effects, as described in the literature, Panksepp continues as follows: “Although many of the behavioral effects could be subsumed by the principle that opioids elaborate pleasure or habit processes in the brain, such a perspective would not explain the peripheral physiological effects of opioids. Suppose we broaden the scheme and postulate that the global function of opioid systems (μ and perhaps δ) is to counteract the influence of stress. Although stress is a construct beset by serious operational and conceptual difficulties, if we consider any major perturbation of physiological homeostasis to be a stress, with opioid arousal being a cardinal counteracting influence, most effects reported for the opioid system fall into place” [59]. “The desirable affective effects of opioids can be understood as the psychic component of a brain process that helps return activity in perturbed neural circuits back to normal”. In addition, “there is an arousal component to opioid action in the brain, especially on cells of the mesolimbic DA (dopamine) pathways, which appears to elaborate the euphoric effects of opioids”, possibly to “promote homeostasis-sustaining behaviors”. “Pleasurable opioid arousal invigorates those active post-homeostatic behavior patterns such as rough-and-tumble play, that are expressed fully only when other bodily needs have been fulfilled” [59]. After some final remarks concerning stress-induced-analgesia, Panksepp concludes that “Perhaps stress-induced-analgesia would be more properly called relief-correlated-analgesia”.
Some years later, Herbert summarized the functional aspects of β-END as follows: this peptide “is particularly concerned with regulating reproductive physiology and is part of the mechanism whereby reproduction is controlled by (and thus responsive to) various elements in the external environment, including social and physical stress” [38]. Central β-END is released during various forms of stress and “is a prime example of a peptide whose principal function seems to be to inhibit responses in conditions under which they would be disadvantageous” (p 739; emphasis is ours). “β-END-containing systems can be accessed by changes in either the physical or social environment that signal adverse conditions for reproduction. The result of activation of this system is a common one: suppression of reproduction” [38].
At first sight, the comments of these successive reviewers may seem difficult to reconcile. However, taking the idea of a main role for β-END in creating a general state of well-being and pleasure (up to euphoria and trance) as a starting point, the differences between the reviewers tend to disappear. This state can be contrasted with alternative states like stress and a variety of motivational states. As proposed by Panksepp, a variety of stressors may induce perturbed neural circuits, and changes towards an opposite preferred endorphin state can be described, supported by activation of the mesolimbic dopaminergic pathways, creating reward when approaching the desired state. Basically, this is not much different, however, from what happens when an animal returns from a specific motivational state (hunger, thirst or sexual arousal) to a state of satiety and homeostatic balance, after performing the appropriate behavioral sequence [30, 31, 38]. Such motivational states may be the result of serious homeostatic needs, which can be very stressful. Concerning food intake, this may imply that eating may be increased or decreased to reach the preferred weight level. We suggest that β-END influences brain-activity towards a state of balance and well-being. In order to keep the brain in this preferred state, β-END apparently allows the animal to perform feeding and drinking activities (see below) as well as social grooming [76], which relieve stress and may restore a disturbed balance. Breeding activities, however, under non-optimal conditions, that would seriously disturb the state of well-being, are apparently inhibited [38, 77, 78], just like painful stimuli that have to be avoided [71, 72, 79]
Our conclusion concerning the general role of β-END is therefore the following. On the one hand, β-END may allow and stimulate behaviors that normally restore a state of homeostatic balance and well-being, and on the other hand it may inhibit behavioral changes that potentially disturb this preferred state. On the basis of this statement, several interesting and specific questions can be raised about the behavioral effects of β-END, about specific behavioral transitions, (as studied by Herbert et al.) [71, 72, 79–85], as well as the effects of meditation. These will be discussed in the next sections.
Behavioral regulation by β-Endorphin
Regulation of food intake
The POMC neurons in the ARH are strongly involved in the regulation of food intake, at the sensory side equipped with specific receptors and at the effector- side with mechanisms controlling food intake. Most of the POMC-neurons express leptin receptors [86, 87], while processing of the POMC-derived peptides is regulated by energy balance [87–90]. On the effector side, it is fully clear that hypothalamic POMC-neurons play an important role in the regulation of food intake [86, 90–97]. Among the POMC-derivatives, alpha-melanocyte-stimulating hormone (α-MSH) appears to be the main one, exerting an inhibitory effect on food intake as shown in human as well as animal research, via the melanocortin receptor types MC3 and MC4 [90, 92, 94, 95, 98–103]. The role of β-END in food intake regulation appears to be less prominent and more modulatory in character. From the earliest studies on, it became clear that central administration of opiates, among them β-END, had a stimulating effect on food intake [105–112]. The stimulating effects of other neuropeptides, like galanin (GAL), were also mediated by β-END release [113] while on the other hand serotonin seems to play a role in the feeding effects of β-END [114–116]. Several recent findings have complicated the role of β-END in food intake. Its levels in both CSF and plasma were elevated after intake of palatable sucrose solutions in rats [117], while ingestion of a fatty meal induced neuronal activity in the β-END neurons, apparently after oropharyngeal stimuli arriving via the glossopharyngeal nerve [118]. This finding is in agreement with an earlier observation showing that, in the rat, β-END plays a special role in the hedonic preferences for dietary fat [119]. In addition, β-END deficient mice are less willing to work for palatable food, suggesting motivational changes in the appetitive phase [120].
Maybe, the short-term effects of β-END on food intake are different from the long-term effects, since intracerebroventricular (icv) infusion of β-END in rats stimulated food intake but chronic infusion did not sustain these stimulating effects [104]. Recently it was also observed in transgenic mice, that that short- and long term effects on POMC-neurons may be different. The long-term effects seem to be more complementary to the general POMC-effects [120, 121]. After a temporary initial increase, male rats showed a lower level of food intake after a few days of chronic icv β-END administration [122]. This weakly inhibitory effect is in agreement with results obtained from β-END-KO mice, which gained an additional 10 – 15% of body weight compared to wild-type controls. These KO mice showed an increase in food intake without changes in basal metabolic rate. The increased body weight consisted completely of an increased amount of white body fat and was only observed in male mice, not in females [121]. Finally, it has been suggested already in the early nineties [123–126] that the role of β-END in food intake is mainly sustaining, instead of playing an initiating role.
The question that has to be raised now is: How far it is possible to integrate this variety of findings into a general role for β-END in the regulation of food intake? The short-term effects of β-END seem to be most prominent and decrease the appetite-inhibiting effects of α-MSH [104]or a preceding stressor [102] and thereby β-END may play a special role in the appetitive phase of feeding behavior. These effects can be linked to the reward system which induces positive feedback stimuli in the appetitive phase [127–129] to sustain food intake of palatable food [119, 121, 126]. Apparently, “β-END selectively affects a motivational component of reward behavior under non-deprived conditions” and “in the appetitive phase, β-END release increases the incentive value of food as a primary reinforcer” [120]. These behavioral effects certainly contribute to restore a state of well-being, as postulated for a general role of β-END. The question as to how far the long-term effects of β-END, consisting of moderate inhibitory effects on food intake, contribute to long term body homeostasis and weight regulation (only in males), or whether these have to be considered merely as a side-effect of β-END supporting some other POMC-derived peptide(s), remains open for further research. In conclusion, the central effects of β-END on food intake are modulatory and play a main role in the introductory/appetitive, goal-directed phase of feeding behavior. In this phase behavior can be most easily adapted to obtain or preserve a preferred state [30, 31] or to choose an alternative coping strategy.
Sexual behavior
Despite the fact that the effects of opioid administration have frequently been described as reaching an orgasmic state of euphoria, the effects on sexual behavior are generally inhibitory [130]. The acute and long-term effects turned out to be complex [131–133], including those of β-END, which plays a role in male as well as in female sexual behavior. In the male, β-END is involved in the regulatory control of testosterone, via luteinizing hormone (LH) and gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH) mechanisms. The effects are mainly inhibitory [132, 134–137]. The details of the observed effects were, however, complex [131, 133, 138], rather variable, for instance those on ejaculation latencies and of naloxone [134, 139–142]. Also the effects of ejaculation, and erotic stimuli (in humans) did not lead to consistent results when measuring the peripheral β-END levels [132, 143–147].
Similar to what we have discussed for the control of feeding behavior, several findings suggest that β-END plays an important role only in specific phases of sexual behavior, especially the precopulatory, appetitive phase. In 1981 Meyerson observed that after icv administration amicable contacts between animals increased whereas sexual responses were decreased [148]. Later studies supported a specific role of the μ-receptors in pair-bonding [149]. Numerous studies have reported the deteriorating effects of stressors on male sexual performance [150–155]. In addition, the analgesic effect of copulatory activities has been noted repeatedly [147, 156, 157]. All findings, taken together, strongly suggest that the main role of β-END is played in the appetitive, precopulatory phase of sexual behavior, to pave the way for the copulatory activities themselves. In this phase, the dopaminergic system is also involved, providing a rewarding basis for the ongoing activities. The effects of β-END during the appetitive arousal state are related to its stimulating effects on the reward systems and an important role, especially in the medial preoptic area (MPOA) [130, 131, 133, 158–172]. In 2004, it was shown that sexual behavior and especially sex-associated environmental cues activate the mesolimbic system in male rats. This activation induces internalization of μ-receptors in the MPOA within 30 minutes after mating and this internalization was still evident about 6 hours later [168, 173]. Naloxone prevented this internalization but not the concurrent Fos- expression of the MPOA-neurons [173]. In 2007, it was shown convincingly that POMC-neurons in rats were activated by the arousal aspects and not by sexual activities themselves [167].
These findings are in full agreement with earlier suggestions concerning the effects of β-END on interpretation and impact of environmental stimuli [174, 175]. Moreover, these findings suggest that the neural mechanisms involved in either the arousal/precopulatory phase or the copulatory phase of male sexual behavior show some fundamental differences. In the rat, an initial appetitive, precopulatory phase of approaching and investigating the female, is usually followed by a sequence of copulatory activities (mounts and intromissions) eventually leading to an ejaculation [176]. After a post-ejaculatory interval of several minutes, the whole behavioral sequence may start again. (See Figure 1 for an overview of the behavioral sequence.) β-END seems to play its specific role especially in the appetitive arousal phase (and in the post-ejaculatory period), but not during the copulatory activities themselves.
Support for the idea of ‘phase-specific’ effects of β-END was obtained by the group of Herbert [38]. They injected β-END bilaterally by micro infusion via brain cannulas into several specific rat brain areas and made some striking observations. After showing the generally inhibiting effects of β-END and the involvement of different brain areas [80–82], it was observed that the inhibitory mechanisms of the MPOA and the medial amygdala (MeA) were very different, but had eventually the same effect. In both brain areas a specific behavioral transition in the usual sequence of (pre)copulatory events turned out to be of crucial importance: the transition between the appetitive, precopulatory, phase and the copulatory phase turned out to be important for the β-END control. After β-END administration into the MPOA, investigative activities were normal but the males never entered the copulatory phase, unless the β-END administration started after the initiation of the copulatory activities. In that case, these were performed as usual. When, however, a new female was introduced, the same precopulatory-copulatory transition was blocked again [38, 85]. On the other hand, the same behavioral transition turned out to play a similar important role in the MeA, but in a completely different way. Now the precopulatory phase was completely suppressed by the β-END administration and the animals never made the transition to the copulatory phase. However, copulatory activities themselves were completely normal, as observed when β-END was administered in the later copulatory phase [38, 83, 84]. These transition-effects were not caused by sensory (olfactory or visual) disturbances possibly induced by the β-END infusions [38].
Apparently, the transition step between precopulatory and copulatory phases of masculine sexual behavior is under β-END control and copulation can be effectively blocked by either prevention or an endless continuation of investigative activities. Since the MeA, especially its posterodorsal part, receives genitosensory as well as olfactory information, and since it is reciprocally connected to the MPOA [31, 32, 177–185], this part of the neural circuitry not only plays a role in the induction of the post-ejaculatory interval, but may just as well be involved in the general control of ejaculatory activities. Manipulation of a specific but crucial behavioral transition is an extremely efficient way to induce or block the occurrence of specific parts of behavioral sequences! Herbert concluded from these and other studies that β-END “is a prime example of a peptide whose principal function seems to be to inhibit responses in conditions under which they would be disadvantageous” [38] (p 739). In our view, β-END appears to play a dual modulatory role in male sexual behavior. On the one hand it facilitates the appetitive phase by reducing stress and potential pain and by activating the reward system [130, 131, 133, 156, 165], on the other hand by inhibitory effects on specific transitions in the regular copulatory pattern in addition to short-term- (post-ejaculatory refractory period) or long-term- (stress, seasonal effects) suppression of reproductive activities. Since the copulatory phase itself is hardly influenced by β-END, these behavioral effects fit a more general anti-stress-function: facilitating behavior towards a state of well-being, reward and even euphoria but inhibiting behavior under inappropriate, potentially stressful, conditions. This dual role of β-END on male sexual behavior is rather similar to what we concluded about its role in food intake.
In the female, β-END mainly has an inhibitory effect on receptivity, lordosis behavior and reproduction [40, 132, 186–193]. This inhibition occurs via the GnRH system in the medial preoptic area, which receives numerous β-END contacts [137, 193–211], as well as via additional μ-receptive MPOA neurons [137, 168, 212–217]. Obviously, the inhibitory effects are estrogen-dependent [168, 193, 218–222], but additional neuroactive substances are also involved in its regulatory control, like neuropeptide Y (NPY), GAL, serotonin (5-HT) and GABA [168, 195, 223, 224]. Interestingly, however, icv-studies showed that β-END could have also a facilitating effect on lordosis, given the proper conditions and location. In ovariectomized rats, primed with estrogen (and progesterone) lordosis in response to male mounts was only inhibited via high-affinity μ-receptors, but facilitated via low affinity δ-receptors [132, 186]. The facilitating effects of β-END were restricted to the first 6 hours after estrogen administration [193–199]. This facilitatory effect changed into an inhibitory effect over the next 6 hours of estradiol-benzoate (EB)-priming [193, 196–199, 225]. The mechanisms involved were determined to be not only time-dependent but also location-dependent. If crystalline EB was implanted in the septal-preoptic regions, β-END effects were facilitatory, if implanted into the ventromedial hypothalamus the EB-implant had an inhibitory effect, while at the level of the mesencephalic reticular formation no effects were observed [225]. In 1997, Gorzalka and coworkers showed that the effects of β-END administration on lordosis were ventricle-dependent: in the lateral ventricle it worked facilitating, but in the 3rd ventricle it had an inhibitory effect, probably due to the activation of different populations of opioid receptors [226]. This observation is reminiscent of other location-dependent effects, as observed by [225] but Gorzalka et al. also noted that β-END had no effect on proceptive behavior, like ear-wiggling [226]. This observation is especially interesting, because it suggests again a phase-specific effect: that β-END may have differential effects on the early introductory/arousal phases of female sexual activities compared to the succeeding copulatory phase. These differential effects are reminiscent of our earlier discussion for male sexual activities, but it is obvious that additional information is needed. In conclusion, the effects of β-END on female sexual behavior appear to be modulatory, as in the male. Depending on the brain areas affected and the state of the opioid receptors involved as well as the gonadal state of the animal, β-END may have facilitating or inhibiting effects on lordotic behavior. In how far these effects contribute to a state of well-being, as suggested for the male sexual activities, deserves further experimental attention, but the avoidance of inappropriate breeding conditions, as hypothesized by Herbert [38] certainly contributes to this state.
Reward and meditation
β-END is known to induce euphoria and to have rewarding and reinforcing properties [71, 227, 228]. Numerous recent reviews discussed the involvement of mu-receptors in the liking and wanting aspects of food reward as well their role in a variety of eating disorders [229–246]. Concerning the rewarding aspects of sexual behavior and the involvement of opioids, a similar series of papers and reviews is available to support this functional relationship [166, 167, 247–254]. The bidirectional interactions between the opioid systems, including β-END, and the mesolimbic (and incerto-hypothalamic) dopaminergic systems compose the neural substrate for the rewarding effects of eating and sexual behavior. These interactions can be considered as crucial components of the mechanisms involved in motivational drives and goal-directed behavior. The motivational effects of numerous neuroactive substances are mere reflections of their inhibitory or excitatory actions on this dopaminergic reward system, extending between the ventral tegmental area (VTA), the nucleus accumbens and the MPOA. “The (induction of a) reward state in males and females is mediated by opioids and the medial preoptic area of the anterior hypothalamus is a crucial site for sexual reward” [249, 255].
In addition to the natural rewards obtained by specific behavioral actions, numerous drugs are able to influence this reward-system directly or indirectly without any specific behavioral activity. These substances induce drug-seeking behavior and addiction with all of their deleterious consequences for the individual and society [227, 228, 240, 256–259]. β-END plays an eminent role in addiction because of its mutual modulatory relationships with the mesolimbic dopaminergic system [256, 268, 260–267]. Its rewarding role in cocaine, alcohol and nicotine addiction is fully supported by the presently available evidence, while the evidence for a role in addiction of tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), the psychoactive component of marijuana, seems to be more circumstantial as yet [228, 257, 258, 261, 268]. Concerning addiction-related stress control, β-END seems to play a prominent but complicated role in the successive phases involved in addiction [228]. While enhancing the rewarding properties of the addiction-related behaviors, β-END diminishes the activity of the stress-related circuitry, (involving the locus coeruleus and the CRH-neurons in the paraventricular hypothalamic nucleus), induced by the anxiogenic side-effects of cocaine [269–273] or by unpredictable distress [273, 274]. The extinction-phase, as a stressful transition between the maintenance- and the withdrawal phases [228], induces again a tremendous increase in β-END release in the nucleus accumbens [276]. During the withdrawal phase, drug desire (craving) remains high while levels of β-END are steadily decreasing [228, 268, 277–279].
Generally speaking, the role of β-END in reward and addiction can be described as follows: on the one hand it may enhance the initial rewarding properties of the (new) behavior or drug, while on the other hand it softens the stressing side-effects of the drug use or other aspects of the addiction-related behavior. Maybe these roles can be considered as the two sides of the same coin, because of the mutually-inhibiting effects of the reward circuitry versus the stress circuitry [280]. However, several findings suggest that the relationship between stress and reward is more complicated than a matter of mutually inhibitory circuitry. In fact, the relationship shows a remarkable similarity to the biochemical aspects of the adrenal stress response, where adrenalin serves the function of making all bodily reserves available for handling the challenge, while at almost the same moment the corticosteroids start their, partially counteracting, anabolic activities for restoring the homeostatic balance in order to be able to cope with future challenges [281, 282]. We assume that a similar balanced relationship is also effective with β-END if processes related to addiction activate the stress-circuitry.
Generally, the neuronal circuitry activated by stressors involves such brain areas (or parts of) as the nucleus of the solitary tract, the parabrachial nuclei, the locus coeruleus, the central amygdaloid nucleus, the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus, the hippocampal formation and cortical areas such as the insular and anterior cingulate regions. Depending on the nature of the stressor, physical or psychogenic, the brain areas mentioned may play a more or a less prominent role in activating the neurons of the pituitary-adrenal axis via the corticotropin-releasing-hormone (CRH-) neurons in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus [283–285]. For example, in the case of physical illness, the lower brainstem areas may play a more prominent role than during chronic, psychogenic stress [286–290]. To complicate matters even one step further: substances like ethanol also play a role in these interactions, as they have been shown to influence anxious behavior, behavioral despair and the effects of stressors on the tail-suspension test or on novelty-suppressed feeding in mice [102, 281, 291]. These interaction-effects are too complicated to discuss them further here, but they show convincingly that β-END is directly or indirectly involved in a range of behavioral effects (feeding behavior) and physiological effects (adrenal size).
Apparently, a CRH-dominated stress circuitry is activated under various circumstances. While it has been observed that CRH in the CSF regulates the expression of the μ opioid receptor [292], the nociception related endothelin system is regulated by β-END levels in the CSF [293]. Interestingly for the present review, however, are the numerous observations showing that activation of the CRH-neurons induces almost immediate activation of β-END neurons, inhibiting further release of CRH [268, 269, 271–273, 294–319]. This suggests that, similar to the ameliorating effects of the corticosteroids on the release of adrenalin, β-END is invoked immediately to modulate the maladaptive effects of the neural stress response. Activation of a rewarding feel-better-circuitry would be most effective to counterbalance stressor-effects, not only for suppressing pain (see below) if necessary to escape a predator, but also to cope with minor daily and repetitive challenges. Apparently, the neural circuitry involved is a complex mix of neuronal and CSF-signals.
Meditation has been shown to be very effective to counterbalance stress effects throughout the ages and its effects clearly stimulate the levels of β-END [296, 297, 307]. The central CSF-levels have not been measured yet under these circumstances. In a recent paper, however, studying ecstatic meditation using functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and electroencephalographic techniques, it was observed that not only superficial cortical brain areas are involved in the effects, but that activation of the reward-system (the dopaminergic fibers contacting the accumbens nucleus) also occurs [294]. This suggests strongly that central parts of the CNS are involved in the effects of meditation, but additional supporting evidence is certainly needed. In the last 2 years, more than 100 papers have appeared about the effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction, that is inspired by vipassana meditation coming from Buddhism, on a variety of symptoms related to chronic pain, anxiety disorders, depression versus well-being and several other medical symptoms like fatigue, fibromyalgia and insomnia [306, 309–326]. Although these stress-counteracting approaches show large differences in the way mindfulness is practiced [319], and are drifting away from classical mindfulness [327], obvious effects on the neuronal substrate have been observed, using a variety of techniques [314, 319, 328–336]. We conclude that meditation is an effective means to manage stress [280] and that β-END is extensively involved in the balance between the positive and relaxing effects of activation of the reward circuitry and the negative consequences of physical problems, pain and (chronic) stress [337], shifting the balance in a positive direction, in accordance with our hypothesis as developed by Panksepp [59, 338–344]. The euphoric state, with a total neglect of bodily and environmental cues including complete suppression of pain [71, 79] can be considered as the most extreme condition in the regular balance between the brain-states and circuitries related to stress and stress-relief.
Pain control mechanisms
The extremely potent analgesic effects of β-END were discovered early, [350–355]and during the late seventies, eighties and early nineties an extensive series of publications appeared in which CSF-levels of β-END were measured and correlated with the pain levels experienced by patients and experimental animals under a variety of painful conditions. This surge of interest emerged from the finding that the analgesic effects of β-END were obvious only after icv-, and not after iv-administration [348, 351–355] This difference illustrated clearly the relative effects of the rather impenetrable blood–brain-barrier combined with the almost 3 times shorter half-life of β-END in the blood versus in the CSF (about 37 min vs. about 97 min; [355]. Despite the originally high expectations, it turned out that the correlations of central β-END levels with chronic pain states were weak or negligible [356–364] with a few exceptions related to abdominal pain and migraine, where CSF-levels tended to be lower than normal [365–372]. While these essentially negative findings make it impossible to use CSF-levels of β-END as a parameter signaling pain perception, there is no reason to conclude that CSF-β-END levels do not play a role in pain control [373].
From the beginning it was established that not only icv-administration of β-END [354, 355] but also electro- or magnetic stimulation [346, 374–397], electro-acupuncture [380–397] as well as physical exercise or therapy [398, 399] induced strong analgesic effects, by elevating the levels of β-END in the CSF. Since acute painful stimuli also raise these CSF levels considerably [170, 359, 375, 400, 401], it is fully clear that the central release of β-END forms part of an antinociceptive system controlling pain [71, 79, 402, 403]. It has been observed that mindfulness meditation modulates pain perception as well, e.g. [320]. This antinociceptive system includes the arcuate hypothalamic nucleus (ARH), with its content of POMC-neurons, the periaqueductal gray region [390, 404, 405] and several caudal brainstem areas, including the caudal raphe nuclei, from where 5-HT projections descend into the spinal cord [402, 403] Many experimental data show, however, that this system is not limited to a set of (partially reciprocal) neuronal connections but that β-END, released into the CSF to go with the flow [14, 404, 405], plays an important modulating role.
As early as 1982, James Henry argued that the variety of conditions under which β-END is released and the fact that these conditions exert effects on a number of systems together, requires a global activation of opiate receptors throughout the CNS [71]. At the time, blood-borne opioid hormone, released by the pituitary or a pituitary-controlled peripheral gland to enter the CNS and the CSF, appeared to be the most appropriate candidate to induce such a generalized response [71, 79]. Later studies, however, showed that β-END can also be released locally in peripheral tissues, to control (local) pain [177, 179, 181, 406–408]. Peripheral levels of β-END may influence the spinal cord [5, 71], but brain-CSF levels arise from the arcuate hypothalamic nucleus (ARH) as well as from numerous terminals surrounding the ventricular spaces and are sufficiently high to induce the central effects. The following findings provide additional evidence.
Extracellular levels of β-END in the ARH show two- to fourfold increases upon painful or 5-HT stimulation. While this release may potentially activate all neighboring POMC neurons, the destination will be the adjoining CSF [404, 405], to ‘go with the flow’. The flowing CSF may collect additional POMC and β-END from the many varicose fibers running subependymally alongside the ventricular system. It is therefore not surprising that high levels of β-END have been measured in the CSF after stimulating its release by a painful or an electrical stimulus [170, 374, 375, 377, 409–412] CSF-levels increased 20-fold, after this central release and the analgesic effects were clearly correlated with the duration and the increase of the levels. In 2001 Shen [379] reported an elucidating rabbit experiment. CSF from one animal, after 30 min of electro-acupuncture, was infused into the lateral ventricle of a naïve recipient rabbit. The analgesic effect was observed in the recipient rabbit, showing that volume transmission via the CSF can indeed be effective. More recently, Zubrzycka and Janecka, showed in an experiment involving both tooth pulp- and central gray electrical stimulation, that β-END was released into the CSF after tooth pulp stimulation and this release could be inhibited by the PAG-stimulation [405, 412]. They concluded that ‘endogenous β-END, released as a result of electrical tooth pulp stimulation in orofacial pain, diffuses through the cerebroventricular ependyma into the CSF and exerts a modulatory effect, mediated by μ-receptors, altering the properties of neurons in the trigeminal sensory nuclei, interneurons, and motoneurons of the hypoglossal nerve’ [405].
Additional evidence comes from intranasal (IN-) application studies, showing that a large variety of substances follow direct nose-to-brain pathways to enter the brain cavity and the CSF compartment, generally within a few minutes [22]. For β-END itself the effects of IN-administration have been hardly reported, probably because of its adverse effects on the nasal epithelium [413, 414] Only in an older monkey study clear-cut effects of IN-β-END were reported, in this case on induced prolactin levels [415]. Several substances supposed to elevate central β-END levels: desmopressin, [416] and calcitonin [417, 418] have been tried with variable results and without measurement of CSF-levels. Intranasally applied morphine, however, has been shown to reach the ventricular system of rodents within minutes with a clear distribution advantage over the intravenous and especially oral administration levels [419, 420]. Peripheral levels apparently played no role in the elevated brain levels. Several substances are currently applied intranasally for clinical pain relief, especially fentanyl [416, 421–426] and the opioid system and the CSF as the transport medium seem to be always involved in the effects of such substances. In addition to mechanisms introducing substances into the CSF, it has also been observed that specific ependymal cells and other neurons are able to take up and transport specific substances from the CSF, towards the soma of neurons, frequently located far away from the ventricular surfaces, where they may elicit responses leading to changes in gene expression [427–429].
Already mentioned were some studies in which transport via the CSF seems to be the only possible explanation of the observed effects. Yadid et al. observed a strong reduction of pain behaviors after transplantation of adrenal medullary cells into the subarachnoid space of the spinal cord [430, 431] but there were several reasons to assume that the observed effects were not the local effects of the transplant. In 2000, Yadid et al. showed the involvement of central β-END mechanisms and the arcuate nucleus in the observed analgesia, apparently by releasing β-END via the CSF as a transporter of messages [411]. In another interesting study, Finegold et al. transferred genes to the meninges surrounding the spinal cord, upon which pia mater cells started to produce β-END [432]. This pia mater-release of β-END had a clear analgesic effect on an inflammation model of persistent pain. Their paracrine paradigm for the treatment of chronic pain shows that substances released in the CSF become functional when transported via the CSF. In conclusion, β-END plays a complicated but major role in the mechanisms controlling pain, and transport via the CSF (volume transmission) forms an essential part in these effects [14]. This role is in full agreement with the rewarding and anti-stress roles put forward in the preceding sections of this review.
Other effects of β-END
In order to limit the scope of the present review, additional behavioral, functional and clinical effects of β-END will not be discussed in detail. Such effects have been reported, for example, on the mechanisms of intestinal transit [439] cardiovascular control [103, 122, 434] and the growth and metastasis of mammary tumor cells [435], but also on the immune system, complex arthritic inflammatory syndromes, fibromyalgia and cerebral infarction [180, 271, 436–448]. An extra complication is that the control and effects of β-END are extensively related to a variety of other humoral and neuromodulatory factors, like serotonin, involved in syndrome such as schizophrenia and depression [449, 450]. For that reason, Hegadoren et al. called attention to a possible role of β-END in the pathophysiology of major depression [337]. They reviewed the multiple interactive links between serotonin, β-END and the HPA-axis involved in major depression. Such interactions are of great importance in view of the hypothesis that β-END is involved in anti-stress mechanisms and well-being. Perhaps the involvement of β-END in depression gives this mind-state a considerable, but undesirable, degree of stability. In that case, the stability of a depression has to be considered as an unpleasant side effect. A discussion of these possibilities is beyond the scope of the present review.
One interaction effect deserves some special attention, namely with the neuropeptide oxytocin (OT), because this peptide plays a major role in positive social interactions and because of it may be released into the CSF to influence distant brain areas by going with the flow [3, 4, 22]. In 1989 Melrose and Knigge studied horse brains and proposed evolutionary relationships for POMC, oxytocin (OT) and vasopressin (AVP) neurons, all of them surrounding the ventricular system and equipped with an extensive set of mutual connections [451]. The remarkable co-existence of opiocortin and corticotropin-releasing factor immunoreactive CRF-IR projections surrounding the ventricular system, as observed from the earliest studies [452, 453], contributes to the suggestion of mutual interaction effects. The fact that each of these neuropeptides shows facilitatory or inhibitory effects on behavior as well as physiological mechanisms like feeding, sex, aggression, pain, reward, (anti)stress and social relationships like maternal and pair-bonding behavior, makes the conclusion unavoidable that these neuropeptides must operate in close mutual interactive relationships. Many behavioral and physiological studies support the existence of these functional relationships [14, 182, 454–479]. Concerning the focus of the present review, central mechanisms using the CSF for transport, readers are also referred to [3–5, 14, 22, 480, 481].
Conclusions
Summarizing the effects of β-END on brain and behavior as described in the literature, they seem to be separable into 2 categories. On the one hand, β-END, released into the CSF, to go with the flow, may have far-reaching effects on distant brain regions involved in a variety of behaviors, related to reward mechanisms and motivational and mental states. This is the global effect with a tendency towards stress-reduction, leading to a sense of well-being by homeostatic balance and behavioral stability. On the other hand, and in addition to this state-transition- effect, local administration of β-END in specific brain areas like amygdala or hypothalamus induces specific inhibitory effects on transitions of the behavioral sequence thereby preventing the occurrence of a specific goal. A funnel-model has been introduced to describe the successive phases of a behavioral sequence. The question as to how far the specific behavioral effects always support the global effect or have to be considered as specific local mechanisms controlling specific behavioral sequences needs further attention and research. Additional experiments with local manipulation of β-END levels in specific brain regions are needed to shed more light on the complex global and specific effects of β-END on the CNS, on behavior and behavioral transitions.
Abbreviations
- α-MSH:
-
Alpha-melanocyte-stimulating hormone
- ARH:
-
Arcuate hypothalamic nucleus
- AVP:
-
Vasopressin
- β-END:
-
Beta-endorphin
- CRH:
-
Corticotropin-releasing hormone
- CNS:
-
Central nervous system
- CSF:
-
Cerebrospinal fluid
- EB:
-
Estradiol benzoate
- GABA:
-
Gamma-aminobutyric acid
- GAL:
-
Galanin
- GnRH:
-
Gonadotropin-releasing hormone
- HPA-:
-
Hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal-
- 5-HT:
-
Serotonin
- icv:
-
Intracerebroventricular
- IN-:
-
Intranasal-
- -IR:
-
-immunoreactive
- KO-:
-
Knock-out-
- LH:
-
Luteinizing hormone
- MC3/4:
-
Melanocortin receptor types
- MeA:
-
Medial amygdala
- MPOA:
-
Medial preoptic area
- NPY:
-
Neuropeptide Y
- OT:
-
Oxytocin
- PAG:
-
Periaqueductal gray
- POMC:
-
Proopiomelanocortin
- THC:
-
Tetrahydrocannabinol
- VMH:
-
Ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus
- VT:
-
Volume transmission.
References
Turing A: On computable numbers, with an application to theEntscheidungsproblem. Proc London Mathematical Soc, Series 2 1937,42(1):230–65.
Salzman CD, Fusi S: Emotion, cognition, and mental state representation in amygdala and prefrontal cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 2010, 33:173–202.
Veening JG, Barendregt HP: The regulation of brain states by neuroactive substances distributed via the cerebrospinal fluid; a review. Cerebrospinal Fluid Res 2010, 7:1.
Veening JG, de Jong T, Barendregt HP: Oxytocin-messages via the cerebrospinal fluid: behavioral effects; a review. Physiol Behav 2010, 101:193–210.
Veening JG, Gerrits PO, Barendregt HP: Volume transmission of beta-endorphin via the cerebrospinal fluid; a review. Fluids Barriers CNS 2012, 9:16.
Agnati LF, Bjelke B, Fuxe K: Volume versus wiring transmission in the brain: a new theoretical frame for neuropsychopharmacology. Med Res Rev 1995, 15:33–45.
Agnati LF, Cortelli P, Biagini G, Bjelke B, Fuxe K: Different classes of volume transmission signals exist in the central nervous system and are affected by metabolic signals, temperature gradients and pressure waves. Neuroreport 1994, 6:9–12.
Agnati LF, Fuxe K: Volume transmission as a key feature of information handling in the central nervous system possible new interpretative value of the Turing’s B-type machine. Prog Brain Res 2000, 125:3–19.
Agnati LF, Fuxe K, Zoli M, Ozini I, Toffano G, Ferraguti F: A correlation analysis of the regional distribution of central enkephalin and beta-endorphin immunoreactive terminals and of opiate receptors in adult and old male rats. Evidence for the existence of two main types of communication in the central nervous system: the volume transmission and the wiring transmission. Acta Physiol Scand 1986, 128:201–7.
Fuxe K, Borroto-Escuela DO, Romero-Fernandez W, Ciruela F, Manger P, Leo G, et al.: On the role of volume transmission and receptor-receptor interactions in social behaviour: focus on central catecholamine and oxytocin neurons. Brain Res 2012, 1476:119–31.
Fuxe K, Rivera A, Jacobsen KX, Hoistad M, Leo G, Horvath TL, et al.: Dynamics of volume transmission in the brain. Focus on catecholamine and opioid peptide communication and the role of uncoupling protein 2. J Neural Transm 2005, 112:65–76.
Agnati LF, Guidolin D, Guescini M, Genedani S, Fuxe K: Understanding wiring and volume transmission. Brain Res Rev 2010, 64:137–59.
Agnati LF, Leo G, Zanardi A, Genedani S, Rivera A, Fuxe K, et al.: Volume transmission and wiring transmission from cellular to molecular networks: history and perspectives. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 2006, 187:329–44.
Fuxe K, Borroto-Escuela DO, Romero-Fernandez W, Zhang WB, Agnati LF: Volume transmission and its different forms in the central nervous system. Chin J Integr Med 2013, 19:323–9.
Fuxe K, Dahlstrom AB, Jonsson G, Marcellino D, Guescini M, Dam M, et al.: The discovery of central monoamine neurons gave volume transmission to the wired brain. Prog Neurobiol 2010, 90:82–100.
Agnati LF, Zoli M, Stromberg I, Fuxe K: Intercellular communication in the brain: wiring versus volume transmission. Neuroscience 1995, 69:711–26.
Malpaux B, Daveau A, Maurice-Mandon F, Duarte G, Chemineau P: Evidence that melatonin acts in the premammillary hypothalamic area to control reproduction in the ewe: presence of binding sites and stimulation of luteinizing hormone secretion by in situ microimplant delivery. Endocrinology 1998, 139:1508–16.
Skinner DC, Malpaux B: High melatonin concentrations in third ventricular cerebrospinal fluid are not due to Galen vein blood recirculating through the choroid plexus. Endocrinology 1999, 140:4399–405.
Sliwowska JH, Billings HJ, Goodman RL, Coolen LM, Lehman MN: The premammillary hypothalamic area of the ewe: anatomical characterization of a melatonin target area mediating seasonal reproduction. Biol Reprod 2004, 70:1768–75.
Tricoire H, Locatelli A, Chemineau P, Malpaux B: Melatonin enters the cerebrospinal fluid through the pineal recess. Endocrinology 2002, 143:84–90.
Tricoire H, Moller M, Chemineau P, Malpaux B: Origin of cerebrospinal fluid melatonin and possible function in the integration of photoperiod. Reprod Suppl 2003, 61:311–21.
Veening JG, Olivier B: Intranasal administration of oxytocin: behavioral and clinical effects, a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013, 37:1445–65.
Barendregt H, Raffone A: Conscious cognition as a discrete, deterministic, and universal Turing Machine process. In The selected works of AM Turing. Edited by: Cooper B, van Leeuwen J. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2013:92–7.
Teuscher C: Turing’s Connectionism. An investigation of Neural Network Architectures. London: Springer-Verlag; 2002.
Turing AM: Intelligent machinery. In Machine intelligence. Volume 5. Edited by: Meltzer B, Michie D. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press; 1969:3–23.
Zylberberg A, Dehaene S, Roelfsema PR, Sigman M: The human Turing machine: a neural framework for mental programs. Trends in Cognnitive Sci 2011, 15:293–300.
VanRullen R, Koch C: Is perception discrete or continuous? Trends in Cognnitive Sci 2003, 7:207–13.
Pascual-Marqui RD, Michel RD, Lehmann D: Segmentation of brain electrical activity into microstates. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 1995, 42:658–65.
Veening JG: Behavioural functions of the VMH in the rat: an ethological approach. the Netherlands, Zoology: University of Groningen; 1975.
Veening JG: Brain and behaviour: morphological and functional aspects of the hypothalamus in the rat. Eur J Morphol 1992, 30:53–66.
Veening JG, Coolen LM: Neural mechanisms of sexual behavior in the male rat: Emphasis on ejaculation-related circuits. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2014, 121:170–83.
Coolen LM: Neural control of ejaculation. J Comp Neurol 2005, 493:39–45.
Coolen LM, Allard J, Truitt WA, McKenna KE: Central regulation of ejaculation. Physiol Behav 2004, 83:203–15.
Coolen LM, Hull EM: Male sexual function. Physiol Behav 2004, 83:175–6.
Ahmed B, Kastin AJ, Banks WA, Zadina JE: CNS effects of peptides: a cross-listing of peptides and their central actions published in the journal Peptides, 1986–1993. Peptides 1994, 15:1105–55.
Bodnar RJ: Endogenous opiates and behavior: 2011. Peptides 2012, 38:463–522.
Font L, Lujan MA, Pastor R: Involvement of the endogenous opioid system in the psychopharmacological actions of ethanol: the role of acetaldehyde. Front Behav Neurosci 2013, 7:93.
Herbert J: Peptides in the limbic system: neurochemical codes for co-ordinated adaptive responses to behavioural and physiological demand. Prog Neurobiol 1993, 41:723–91.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1982. Peptides 1983, 4:563–76.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1983. Peptides 1984, 5:975–92.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1984. Peptides 1985, 6:769–91.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1985. Peptides 1986, 7:907–33.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1986. Peptides 1987, 8:1135–64.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1988. Peptides 1989, 10:1253–80.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1987. Peptides 1989, 10:205–36.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1989. Peptides 1990, 11:1277–304.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1990. Peptides 1991, 12:1407–32.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1991. Peptides 1992, 13:1247–87.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1992. Peptides 1993, 14:1339–78.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1993. Peptides 1994, 15:1513–56.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1994. Peptides 1995, 16:1517–55.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1995. Peptides 1996, 17:1421–66.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1996. Peptides 1997, 18:1651–88.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ, Coy DH: Endogenous opiates: through 1978. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1979, 3:285–99.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ, Coy DH: Endogenous opiates: 1979(1). Peptides 1980, 1:365–79.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ, Coy DH: Endogenous opiates: 1980. Peptides 1981, 2:349–69.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ, Coy DH: Endogenous opiates: 1981. Peptides 1982, 3:1039–72.
Olson GA, Olson RD, Vaccarino AL, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1997. Peptides 1998, 19:1791–843.
Panksepp J: The neurochemistry of behavior. Annu Rev Psychol 1986, 37:77–107.
Roy A, Roy M, Deb S, Unwin G, Roy A: Are opioid antagonists effective in attenuating the core symptoms of autism spectrum conditions in children: a systematic review. J Intellect Disabil Res 2014. doi: 10.1111/jir.12122. [Epub ahead of print] PubMed PMID: 24589346.
Sarkar DK, Zhang C: Beta-endorphin neuron regulates stress response and innate immunity to prevent breast cancer growth and progression. Vitam Horm 2013, 93:263–76.
Vaccarino AL, Olson GA, Olson RD, Kastin AJ: Endogenous opiates: 1998. Peptides 1999, 20:1527–74.
Zhao K: Acupuncture for the treatment of insomnia. Int Rev Neurobiol 2013, 111:217–34.
Benedetti F: Placebo and endogenous mechanisms of analgesia. Handb Exp Pharmacol 2007, (177):393–413. Review. PubMed PMID: 17087131
Butcher BE, Carmody JJ: Sex differences in analgesic response to ibuprofen are influenced by expectancy: a randomized, crossover, balanced placebo-designed study. Eur J Pain 2012, 16:1005–13.
Jaksic N, Aukst-Margetic B, Jakovljevic M: Does personality play a relevant role in the placebo effect? Psychiatr Danub 2013, 25:17–23.
Lemoine P: The placebo mystery or neurobiology of the soul. Bull Acad Natl Med 2011, 195:1465–76.
Mommaerts JL, Devroey D: The placebo effect: how the subconscious fits in. Perspect Biol Med 2012, 55:43–58.
Zubieta JK, Stohler CS: Neurobiological mechanisms of placebo responses. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009, 1156:198–210.
Diederich NJ, Goetz CG: The placebo treatments in neurosciences: New insights from clinical and neuroimaging studies. Neurology 2008, 71:677–84.
Henry JL: Circulating opioids: possible physiological roles in central nervous function. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1982, 6:229–45.
Akil H, Watson SJ, Young E, Lewis ME, Khachaturian H, Walker JM: Endogenous opioids: biology and function. Annu Rev Neurosci 1984, 7:223–55.
Izquierdo I, Netto CA: The brain beta-endorphin system and behavior: the modulation of consecutively and simultaneously processed memories. Behav Neural Biol 1985, 44:249–65.
Izquierdo I, Netto CA: Role of beta-endorphin in behavioral regulation. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1985, 444:162–77.
Izquierdo I, Netto CA, Carrasco MA, Dias RD, Volkmer N: The course of the decrease of hypothalamic beta-endorphin induced by training, and the development of the effect of beta-endorphin on the retrieval of inhibitory avoidance in rats. Braz J Med Biol Res 1985, 18:391–5.
Keverne EB, Martensz ND, Tuite B: Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1989, 14:155–61.
Martensz ND, Vellucci SV, Keverne EB, Herbert J: beta-Endorphin levels in the cerebrospinal fluid of male talapoin monkeys in social groups related to dominance status and the luteinizing hormone response to naloxone. Neuroscience 1986, 18:651–8.
Roberts AC, Martensz ND, Hastings MH, Herbert J: The effects of castration, testosterone replacement and photoperiod upon hypothalamic beta-endorphin levels in the male Syrian hamster. Neuroscience 1987, 23:1075–82.
Henry JL: Role of circulating opioids in the modulation of pain. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1986, 467:169–81.
Hughes AM, Everitt BJ, Herbert J: Selective effects of beta-endorphin infused into the hypothalamus, preoptic area and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis on the sexual and ingestive behaviour of male rats. Neuroscience 1987, 23:1063–73.
Hughes AM, Everitt BJ, Herbert J: The effects of simultaneous or separate infusions of some pro-opiomelanocortin-derived peptides (beta-endorphin, melanocyte stimulating hormone, and corticotrophin-like intermediate polypeptide) and their acetylated derivatives upon sexual and ingestive behaviour of male rats. Neuroscience 1988, 27:689–98.
Hughes AM, Everitt BJ, Herbert J: Comparative effects of preoptic area infusions of opioid peptides, lesions and castration on sexual behaviour in male rats: studies of instrumental behaviour, conditioned place preference and partner preference. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1990, 102:243–56.
McGregor A, Herbert J: The effects of beta-endorphin infusions into the amygdala on visual and olfactory sensory processing during sexual behaviour in the male rat. Neuroscience 1992, 46:173–9.
McGregor A, Herbert J: Specific effects of beta-endorphin infused into the amygdala on sexual behaviour in the male rat. Neuroscience 1992, 46:165–72.
Stavy M, Herbert J: Differential effects of beta-endorphin infused into the hypothalamic preoptic area at various phases of the male rat’s sexual behaviour. Neuroscience 1989, 30:433–42.
Mercer AJ, Stuart RC, Attard CA, Otero-Corchon V, Nillni EA, Low MJ: Temporal changes in nutritional state affect hypothalamic POMC peptide levels independently of leptin in adult male mice. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 2014, 306:E904–15.
Cheung CC, Clifton DK, Steiner RA: Proopiomelanocortin neurons are direct targets for leptin in the hypothalamus. Endocrinology 1997, 138:4489–92.
Pritchard LE, White A: Neuropeptide processing and its impact on melanocortin pathways. Endocrinology 2007, 148:4201–7.
Pritchard LE, Oliver RL, McLoughlin JD, Birtles S, Lawrence CB, Turnbull AV, et al.: Proopiomelanocortin-derived peptides in rat cerebrospinal fluid and hypothalamic extracts: evidence that secretion is regulated with respect to energy balance. Endocrinology 2003, 144:760–6.
Millington GW: The role of proopiomelanocortin (POMC) neurones in feeding behaviour. Nutr Metab (Lond) 2007, 4:18.
Ma J, Xia J, Miele L, Sarkar FH, Wang Z: Notch signaling pathway in pancreatic cancer progression. Pancreat Disord Ther 2013.,30(114): pii: 1000114. PubMed PMID: 24027656; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3767173
Lee M, Kim A, Conwell IM, Hruby V, Mayorov A, Cai M, et al.: Effects of selective modulation of the central melanocortin-3-receptor on food intake and hypothalamic POMC expression. Peptides 2008, 29:440–7.
Kim MS, Rossi M, Abusnana S, Sunter D, Morgan DG, Small CJ, et al.: Hypothalamic localization of the feeding effect of agouti-related peptide and alpha-melanocyte-stimulating hormone. Diabetes 2000, 49:177–82.
Zheng H, Patterson LM, Rhodes CJ, Louis GW, Skibicka KP, Grill HJ, et al.: A potential role for hypothalamomedullary POMC projections in leptin-induced suppression of food intake. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2010, 298:R720–8.
Seeley RJ, Drazen DL, Clegg DJ: The critical role of the melanocortin system in the control of energy balance. Annu Rev Nutr 2004, 24:133–49.
de Backer MW, la Fleur SE, Brans MA, van Rozen AJ, Luijendijk MC, Merkestein M, et al.: Melanocortin receptor-mediated effects on obesity are distributed over specific hypothalamic regions. Int J Obes (Lond) 2011, 35:629–41.
Werner L, Muller-Fielitz H, Ritzal M, Werner T, Rossner M, Schwaninger M: Involvement of doublecortin-expressing cells in the arcuate nucleus in body weight regulation. Endocrinology 2012, 153:2655–64.
Kim JW, Park TJ, Ryu DD, Kim JY: High cell density culture of Yarrowia lipolytica using a one-step feeding process. Biotechnol Prog 2000, 16:657–60.
Petervari E, Balasko M, Garami A, Soos S, Szekely M: Suppression of food intake by intracerebroventricular injection of alpha-MSH varies with age in rats. Acta Physiol Hung 2009, 96:483–7.
Zheng H, Patterson LM, Phifer CB, Berthoud HR: Brain stem melanocortinergic modulation of meal size and identification of hypothalamic POMC projections. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2005, 289:R247–58.
Dutta J, Tripathi S, Dutta PK: Progress in antimicrobial activities of chitin, chitosan and its oligosaccharides: a systematic study needs for food applications. Food Sci Technol Int 2012, 18:3–34.
Barfield ET, Moser VA, Hand A, Grisel JE: beta-endorphin modulates the effect of stress on novelty-suppressed feeding. Front Behav Neurosci 2013, 7:19.
Skibicka KP, Grill HJ: Hypothalamic and hindbrain melanocortin receptors contribute to the feeding, thermogenic, and cardiovascular action of melanocortins. Endocrinology 2009, 150:5351–61.
Dutia R, Meece K, Dighe S, Kim AJ, Wardlaw SL: beta-Endorphin antagonizes the effects of alpha-MSH on food intake and body weight. Endocrinology 2012, 153:4246–55.
Sanger DJ, McCarthy PS: Increased food and water intake produced in rats by opiate receptor agonists. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1981, 74:217–20.
Sanger DJ, McCarthy PS, Metcalf G: The effects of opiate antagonists on food intake are stereospecific. Neuropharmacology 1981, 20:45–7.
Morley JE, Levine AS, Murray SS, Kneip J: Peptidergic regulation of norepinephrine induced feeding. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1982, 16:225–8.
Morley JE, Levine AS, Plotka ED, Seal US: The effect of naloxone on feeding and spontaneous locomotion in the wolf. Physiol Behav 1983, 30:331–4.
Morley JE, Bartness TJ, Gosnell BA, Levine AS: Peptidergic regulation of feeding. Int Rev Neurobiol 1985, 27:207–98.
Grandison L, Guidotti A: Stimulation of food intake by muscimol and beta endorphin. Neuropharmacology 1977, 16:533–6.
Sanger DJ: Endorphinergic mechanisms in the control of food and water intake. Appetite 1981, 2:193–208.
Morley JE, Levine AS, Grace M, Kniep J: An investigation of the role of kappa opiate receptor agonists in the initiation of feeding. Life Sci 1982, 31:2617–26.
Dube MG, Horvath TL, Leranth C, Kalra PS, Kalra SP: Naloxone reduces the feeding evoked by intracerebroventricular galanin injection. Physiol Behav 1994, 56:811–3.
Wang QP, Guan JL, Nakai Y: Beta-endorphinergic innervation of mu and delta receptor containing neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus. Neuroreport 1998, 9:3033–6.
Wang QP, Ochiai H, Guan JL, Nakai Y: Ultrastructural localization of mu-1 opioid receptor in the dorsal raphe nucleus of the rat. Synapse 1998, 29:240–7.
Robert JJ, Orosco M, Rouch C, Cohen Y, Jacquot C: Effects of dexfenfluramine and opioid peptides, alone or in combination, on food intake and brain serotonin turnover in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1991, 38:775–80.
Yamamoto T, Sako N, Maeda S: Effects of taste stimulation on beta-endorphin levels in rat cerebrospinal fluid and plasma. Physiol Behav 2000, 69:345–50.
Matsumura S, Eguchi A, Okafuji Y, Tatsu S, Mizushige T, Tsuzuki S, et al.: Dietary fat ingestion activates beta-endorphin neurons in the hypothalamus. FEBS Lett 2012, 586:1231–5.
Mizushige T, Saitoh K, Manabe Y, Nishizuka T, Taka Y, Eguchi A, et al.: Preference for dietary fat induced by release of beta-endorphin in rats. Life Sci 2009, 84:760–5.
Low MJ, Hayward MD, Appleyard SM, Rubinstein M: State-dependent modulation of feeding behavior by proopiomelanocortin-derived beta-endorphin. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2003, 994:192–201.
Appleyard SM, Hayward M, Young JI, Butler AA, Cone RD, Rubinstein M, et al.: A role for the endogenous opioid beta-endorphin in energy homeostasis. Endocrinology 2003, 144:1753–60.
Hill C, Lapanowski K, Dunbar JC: The effects of beta-endorphin (beta-END) on cardiovascular and behavioral dynamics in conscious rats. Brain Res Bull 2002, 59:29–34.
Carr KD, Simon EJ: The role of opioids in feeding and reward elicited by lateral hypothalamic electrical stimulation. Life Sci 1983,33(Suppl 1):563–6.
Carr KD, Bak TH: Rostral and caudal ventricular infusion of antibodies to dynorphin A(1–17) and dynorphin A(1–8): effects on electrically-elicited feeding in the rat. Brain Res 1990, 507:289–94.
Carr KD, Wolinsky TD: Regulation of feeding by multiple opioid receptors in cingulate cortex; follow-up to an in vivo autoradiographic study. Neuropeptides 1994, 26:207–13.
Papadouka V, Carr KD: The role of multiple opioid receptors in the maintenance of stimulation-induced feeding. Brain Res 1994, 639:42–8.
de Ruiter L, Wiepkema PR, Veening JG: Models of behavior and the hypothalamus. Prog Brain Res 1974, 41:481–507.
Wiepkema PR: Behavioural factors in the regulation of food intake. Proc Nutr Soc 1971, 30:142–9.
Wiepkema PR: Positive feedbacks at work during feeding. Behaviour 1971, 39:266–73.
Argiolas A, Melis MR: Neuropeptides and central control of sexual behaviour from the past to the present: a review. Prog Neurobiol 2013, 108:80–107.
Mahler SV, Berridge KC: What and when to “want”? Amygdala-based focusing of incentive salience upon sugar and sex. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012, 221:407–26.
Pfaus JG, Gorzalka BB: Opioids and sexual behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1987, 11:1–34.
Pfaus JG, Kippin TE, Coria-Avila GA, Gelez H, Afonso VM, Ismail N, et al.: Who, what, where, when (and maybe even why)? How the experience of sexual reward connects sexual desire, preference, and performance. Arch Sex Behav 2012, 41:31–62.
McIntosh TK, Vallano ML, Barfield RJ: Effects of morphine, beta-endorphin and naloxone on catecholamine levels and sexual behavior in the male rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1980, 13:435–41.
Dornan WA, Malsbury CW: Neuropeptides and male sexual behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1989, 13:1–15.
Meyerson BJ, Terenius L: Beta-endorphin and male sexual behavior. Eur J Pharmacol 1977, 42:191–2.
Teodorov E, Camarini R, Bernardi MM, Felicio LF: Treatment with steroid hormones and morphine alters general activity, sexual behavior, and opioid gene expression in female rats. Life Sci 2014,104((1–2)):47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2014.03.021. Epub 2014 Mar 31. PubMed PMID: 24699004
Parada M, Vargas EB, Kyres M, Burnside K, Pfaus JG: The role of ovarian hormones in sexual reward states of the female rat. Horm Behav 2012, 62:442–7.
Gessa GL, Paglietti E, Quarantotti BP: Induction of copulatory behavior in sexually inactive rats by naloxine. Science 1979, 204:203–5.
Myers BM, Baum MJ: Facilitation by opiate antagonists of sexual performance in the male rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1979, 10:615–8.
Glick BB, Baughman WL, Jensen JN, Phoenix CH: Endogenous opiate systems and primate reproduction: inability of naloxone to induce sexual activity in rhesus males. Arch Sex Behav 1982, 11:267–75.
Fabbri A, Jannini EA, Gnessi L, Moretti C, Ulisse S, Franzese A, et al.: Endorphins in male impotence: evidence for naltrexone stimulation of erectile activity in patient therapy. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1989, 14:103–11.
Bishop JD, Malven PV, Singleton WL, Weesner GD: Hormonal and behavioral correlates of emotional states in sexually trained boars. J Anim Sci 1999, 77:3339–45.
Carani C, Bancroft J, Del Rio G, Granata AR, Facchinetti F, Marrama P: The endocrine effects of visual erotic stimuli in normal men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1990, 15:207–16.
Daly RC, Su TP, Schmidt PJ, Pickar D, Murphy DL, Rubinow DR: Cerebrospinal fluid and behavioral changes after methyltestosterone administration: preliminary findings. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001, 58:172–7.
Szechtman H, Hershkowitz M, Simantov R: Sexual behavior decreases pain sensitivity and stimulated endogenous opioids in male rats. Eur J Pharmacol 1981, 70:279–85.
Forsberg G, Wiesenfeld-Hallin Z, Eneroth P, Sodersten P: Sexual behavior induces naloxone-reversible hypoalgesia in male rats. Neurosci Lett 1987, 81:151–4.
Meyerson BJ: Comparison of the effects of beta-endorphin and morphine on exploratory and socio-sexual behaviour in the male rat. Eur J Pharmacol 1981, 69:453–63.
Burkett JP, Spiegel LL, Inoue K, Murphy AZ, Young LJ: Activation of mu-opioid receptors in the dorsal striatum is necessary for adult social attachment in monogamous prairie voles. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011, 36:2200–10.
McGrady AV: Effects of psychological stress on male reproduction: a review. Arch Androl 1984, 13:1–7.
Taylor GT, Weiss J, Rupich R: Male rat behavior, endocrinology and reproductive physiology in a mixed-sex, socially stressful colony. Physiol Behav 1987, 39:429–33.
Retana-Marquez S, Salazar ED, Velazquez-Moctezuma J: Effect of acute and chronic stress on masculine sexual behavior in the rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1996, 21:39–50.
Sato Y, Suzuki N, Horita H, Wada H, Shibuya A, Adachi H, et al.: Effects of long-term psychological stress on sexual behavior and brain catecholamine levels. J Androl 1996, 17:83–90.
Brotto LA, Gorzalka BB, LaMarre AK: Melatonin protects against the effects of chronic stress on sexual behaviour in male rats. Neuroreport 2001, 12:3465–9.
Cameron N, Erskine MS: c-FOS expression in the forebrain after mating in the female rat is altered by adrenalectomy. Neuroendocrinology 2003, 77:305–13.
Gonzalez-Mariscal G, Gomora P, Beyer C: Participation of opiatergic, GABAergic, and serotonergic systems in the expression of copulatory analgesia in male rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1994, 49:303–7.
D’Amato FR, Pavone F: Modulation of nociception by social factors in rodents: contribution of the opioid system. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012, 224:189–200.
Burgdorf J, Panksepp J: Tickling induces reward in adolescent rats. Physiol Behav 2001, 72:167–73.
Van Ree JM, Niesink RJ, Van Wolfswinkel L, Ramsey NF, Kornet MM, Van Furth WR, et al.: Endogenous opioids and reward. Eur J Pharmacol 2000, 405:89–101.
van Furth WR, van Emst MG, van Ree JM: Opioids and sexual behavior of male rats: involvement of the medial preoptic area. Behav Neurosci 1995, 109:123–34.
Van Furth WR, Van Ree JM: Appetitive sexual behavior in male rats: 2. sexual reward and level-changing behavior. Physiol Behav 1996, 60:1007–12.
van Furth WR, van Ree JM: Sexual motivation: involvement of endogenous opioids in the ventral tegmental area. Brain Res 1996, 729:20–8.
Agmo A, Gomez M: Sexual reinforcement is blocked by infusion of naloxone into the medial preoptic area. Behav Neurosci 1993, 107:812–8.
Mitchell JB, Gratton A: Opioid modulation and sensitization of dopamine release elicited by sexually relevant stimuli: a high speed chronoamperometric study in freely behaving rats. Brain Res 1991, 551:20–7.
Agmo A, Berenfeld R: Reinforcing properties of ejaculation in the male rat: role of opioids and dopamine. Behav Neurosci 1990, 104:177–82.
Balfour ME, Yu L, Coolen LM: Sexual behavior and sex-associated environmental cues activate the mesolimbic system in male rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004, 29:718–30.
Davis BA, Fitzgerald ME, Brown JL, Amstalden KA, Coolen LM: Activation of POMC neurons during general arousal but not sexual behavior in male rats. Behav Neurosci 2007, 121:1012–22.
Sinchak K, Dewing P, Ponce L, Gomez L, Christensen A, Berger M, et al.: Modulation of the arcuate nucleus-medial preoptic nucleus lordosis regulating circuit: a role for GABAB receptors. Horm Behav 2013, 64:136–43.
Agnati LF, Genedani S, Leo G, Forni A, Woods AS, Filaferro M, et al.: Abeta peptides as one of the crucial volume transmission signals in the trophic units and their interactions with homocysteine. Physiological implications and relevance for Alzheimer’s disease. J Neural Transm 2007, 114:21–31.
Bach FW, Yaksh TL: Release into ventriculo-cisternal perfusate of beta-endorphin- and Met-enkephalin-immunoreactivity: effects of electrical stimulation in the arcuate nucleus and periaqueductal gray of the rat. Brain Res 1995, 690:167–76.
van Furth WR, van Ree JM: Endogenous opioids and sexual motivation and performance during the light phase of the diurnal cycle. Brain Res 1994, 636:175–9.
van Furth WR, Wolterink G, van Ree JM: Regulation of masculine sexual behavior: involvement of brain opioids and dopamine. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 1995, 21:162–84.
Coolen LM, Fitzgerald ME, Yu L, Lehman MN: Activation of mu opioid receptors in the medial preoptic area following copulation in male rats. Neuroscience 2004, 124:11–21.
Meyerson BJ, Hoglund AU: Exploratory and socio-sexual behaviour in the male laboratory rat: a methodological approach for the investigation of drug action. Acta Pharmacol Toxicol (Copenh) 1981, 48:168–80.
Wu FM, Noble RG: Opiate antagonists and copulatory behavior of male hamsters. Physiol Behav 1986, 38:817–25.
Truitt WA, Coolen LM: Identification of a potential ejaculation generator in the spinal cord. Science 2002, 297:1566–9.
Alves DP, da Motta PG, Lima PP, Queiroz-Junior CM, Caliari MV, Pacheco DF, et al.: Inflammation mobilizes local resources to control hyperalgesia: the role of endogenous opioid peptides. Pharmacology 2012, 89:22–8.
Imaizumi T, Numata A, Yano C, Yoshida H, Meng P, Hayakari R, et al.: ISG54 and ISG56 are induced by TLR3 signaling in U373MG human astrocytoma cells: Possible involvement in CXCL10 expression. Neurosci Res 2014, 84:34–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2014.03.001. Epub 2014 Mar 11. PubMed PMID: 24630834
Ishikawa T, Miyagi M, Yamashita M, Kamoda H, Eguchi Y, Arai G, et al.: In-vivo transfection of the proopiomelanocortin gene, precursor of endogenous endorphin, by use of radial shock waves alleviates neuropathic pain. J Orthop Sci 2013, 18:636–45.
Nakamoto K, Nishinaka T, Sato N, Mankura M, Koyama Y, Kasuya F, et al.: Hypothalamic GPR40 signaling activated by free long chain fatty acids suppresses CFA-induced inflammatory chronic pain. PLoS One 2013, 8:e81563.
Slominski AT, Zmijewski MA, Skobowiat C, Zbytek B, Slominski RM, Steketee JD: Sensing the environment: regulation of local and global homeostasis by the skin's neuroendocrine system. Adv Anat Embryol Cell Biol 2012, 212:v-vii. 1–115
Yu JS, Zeng BY, Hsieh CL: Acupuncture stimulation and neuroendocrine regulation. Int Rev Neurobiol 2013, 111:125–40.
Zhang Y, Liu B, Ma Y, Yi J, Zhang C, Zhang Y, et al.: Hantaan virus infection induces CXCL10 expression through TLR3, RIG-I, and MDA-5 pathways correlated with the disease severity. Mediators Inflamm 2014, 2014:697837.
Coolen LM, Peters HJ, Veening JG: Fos immunoreactivity in the rat brain following consummatory elements of sexual behavior: a sex comparison. Brain Res 1996, 738:67–82.
Coolen LM, Peters HJ, Veening JG: Anatomical interrelationships of the medial preoptic area and other brain regions activated following male sexual behavior: a combined fos and tract-tracing study. J Comp Neurol 1998, 397:421–35.
Pfaus JG, Gorzalka BB: Selective activation of opioid receptors differentially affects lordosis behavior in female rats. Peptides 1987, 8:309–17.
Pfaus JG, Pendleton N, Gorzalka BB: Dual effect of morphiceptin on lordosis behavior: possible mediation by different opioid receptor subtypes. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1986, 24:1461–4.
Sirinathsinghji DJ: Modulation of lordosis behavior of female rats by naloxone, beta-endorphin and its antiserum in the mesencephalic central gray: possible mediation via GnRH. Neuroendocrinology 1984, 39:222–30.
Sirinathsinghji DJ: Regulation of lordosis behaviour in the female rat by corticotropin-releasing factor, beta-endorphin/corticotropin and luteinizing hormone-releasing hormone neuronal systems in the medial preoptic area. Brain Res 1986, 375:49–56.
Sirinathsinghji DJ, Rees LH, Rivier J, Vale W: Corticotropin-releasing factor is a potent inhibitor of sexual receptivity in the female rat. Nature 1983, 305:232–5.
Wiesner JB, Moss RL: Behavioral specificity of beta-endorphin suppression of sexual behavior: differential receptor antagonism. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1986, 24:1235–9.
Wiesner JB, Moss RL: Suppression of receptive and proceptive behavior in ovariectomized, estrogen-progesterone-primed rats by intraventricular beta-endorphin: studies of behavioral specificity. Neuroendocrinology 1986, 43:57–62.
Kubo K: Modulation of estrogen-activated sexual receptivity by opioid peptides. In Emotions: neuronal and chemical control. Edited by: Oomura Y. Basel: Karger; 1986:267–80.
Sirinathsinghji DJ, Whittington PE, Audsley A, Fraser HM: beta-Endorphin regulates lordosis in female rats by modulating LH-RH release. Nature 1983, 301:62–4.
Mills RH, Sohn RK, Micevych PE: Estrogen-induced mu-opioid receptor internalization in the medial preoptic nucleus is mediated via neuropeptide Y-Y1 receptor activation in the arcuate nucleus of female rats. J Neurosci 2004, 24:947–55.
Torii M, Kubo K, Sasaki T: Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of beta-endorphin on lordosis in female rats: relation to time of administration. Horm Behav 1999, 35:271–8.
Torii M, Kubo K, Sasaki T: Differential effects of beta-endorphin and Met- and Leu-enkephalin on steroid hormone-induced lordosis in ovariectomized female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1997, 58:837–42.
Torii M, Kubo K, Sasaki T: Influence of opioid peptides on the priming action of estrogen on lordosis in ovariectomized rats. Neurosci Lett 1996, 212:68–70.
Torii M, Kubo K, Sasaki T: Naloxone and initial estrogen action to induce lordosis in ovariectomized rats: the effect of a cut between the septum and preoptic area. Neurosci Lett 1995, 195:167–70.
Chen WP, Witkin JW, Silverman AJ: beta-Endorphin and gonadotropin-releasing hormone synaptic input to gonadotropin-releasing hormone neurosecretory cells in the male rat. J Comp Neurol 1989, 286:85–95.
Drouva SV, Epelbaum J, Tapia-Arancibia L, Laplante E, Kordon C: Opiate receptors modulate LHRH and SRIF release from mediobasal hypothalamic neurons. Neuroendocrinology 1981, 32:163–7.
Adler BA, Crowley WR: Modulation of luteinizing hormone release and catecholamine activity by opiates in the female rat. Neuroendocrinology 1984, 38:248–53.
Leranth C, MacLusky NJ, Shanabrough M, Naftolin F: Immunohistochemical evidence for synaptic connections between pro-opiomelanocortin-immunoreactive axons and LH-RH neurons in the preoptic area of the rat. Brain Res 1988, 449:167–76.
Babu GN, Marco J, Bona-Gallo A, Gallo RV: Steroid-independent endogenous opioid peptide suppression of pulsatile luteinizing hormone release between estrus and diestrus in the rat estrous cycle. Brain Res 1987, 416:235–42.
Gallo RV, Babu GN, Bona-Gallo A, Devorshak-Harvey E, Leipheimer RE, Marco J: Regulation of pulsatile luteinizing hormone release during the estrous cycle and pregnancy in the rat. Adv Exp Med Biol 1987, 219:109–30.
Martensz ND: Changes in the processing of beta-endorphin in the hypothalamus and pituitary gland of female rats during sexual maturation. Neuroscience 1985, 16:625–40.
Kawano H, Masuko S: Beta-endorphin-, adrenocorticotrophic hormone- and neuropeptide y-containing projection fibers from the arcuate hypothalamic nucleus make synaptic contacts on to nucleus preopticus medianus neurons projecting to the paraventricular hypothalamic nucleus in the rat. Neuroscience 2000, 98:555–65.
Berglund LA, Simpkins JW: Alterations in brain opiate receptor mechanisms on proestrous afternoon. Neuroendocrinology 1988, 48:394–400.
Berglund LA, Derendorf H, Simpkins JW: Desensitization of brain opiate receptor mechanisms by gonadal steroid treatments that stimulate luteinizing hormone secretion. Endocrinology 1988, 122:2718–26.
Leranth C, MacLusky NJ, Shanabrough M, Naftolin F: Catecholaminergic innervation of luteinizing hormone-releasing hormone and glutamic acid decarboxylase immunopositive neurons in the rat medial preoptic area. An electron-microscopic double immunostaining and degeneration study. Neuroendocrinology 1988, 48:591–602.
Hammer RP Jr, Zhou L, Cheung S: Gonadal steroid hormones and hypothalamic opioid circuitry. Horm Behav 1994, 28:431–7.
Sinchak K, Romeo HE, Micevych PE: Site-specific estrogen and progestin regulation of orphanin FQ/nociceptin and nociceptin opioid receptor mRNA expression in the female rat limbic hypothalamic system. J Comp Neurol 2006, 496:252–68.
Sinchak K, Shahedi K, Dewing P, Micevych P: Sexual receptivity is reduced in the female mu-opioid receptor knockout mouse. Neuroreport 2005, 16:1697–700.
Sinchak K, Micevych P: Visualizing activation of opioid circuits by internalization of G protein-coupled receptors. Mol Neurobiol 2003, 27:197–222.
Micevych PE, Rissman EF, Gustafsson JA, Sinchak K: Estrogen receptor-alpha is required for estrogen-induced mu-opioid receptor internalization. J Neurosci Res 2003, 71:802–10.
Sinchak K, Micevych PE: Progesterone blockade of estrogen activation of mu-opioid receptors regulates reproductive behavior. J Neurosci 2001, 21:5723–9.
Sinchak K, Mills RH, Eckersell CB, Micevych PE: Medial preoptic area delta-opioid receptors inhibit lordosis. Behav Brain Res 2004, 155:301–6.
Jirikowski GF, Merchenthaler I, Rieger GE, Stumpf WE: Estradiol target sites immunoreactive for beta-endorphin in the arcuate nucleus of rat and mouse hypothalamus. Neurosci Lett 1986, 65:121–6.
Thornton JE, Loose MD, Kelly MJ, Ronnekleiv OK: Effects of estrogen on the number of neurons expressing beta-endorphin in the medial basal hypothalamus of the female guinea pig. J Comp Neurol 1994, 341:68–77.
Miller MM, Tousignant P, Yang U, Pedvis S, Billiar RB: Effects of age and long-term ovariectomy on the estrogen-receptor containing subpopulations of beta-endorphin-immunoreactive neurons in the arcuate nucleus of female C57BL/6J mice. Neuroendocrinology 1995, 61:542–51.
Cheung S, Hammer RP Jr: Gonadal steroid hormone regulation of proopiomelanocortin gene expression in arcuate neurons that innervate the medial preoptic area of the rat. Neuroendocrinology 1995, 62:283–92.
Cheung S, Salinas J, Hammer RP Jr: Gonadal steroid hormone-dependence of beta-endorphin-like immunoreactivity in the medial preoptic area of the rat. Brain Res 1995, 675:83–8.
Horvath TL, Kalra SP, Naftolin F, Leranth C: Morphological evidence for a galanin-opiate interaction in the rat mediobasal hypothalamus. J Neuroendocrinol 1995, 7:579–88.
Allen DL, Johnson AE, Tempel A, Zukin RS, Luine VN, McEwen BS: Serotonergic lesions decrease mu- and delta-opiate receptor binding in discrete areas of the hypothalamus and in the midbrain central gray. Brain Res 1993, 625:269–75.
Torii M, Kubo K: The effects of intraventricular injection of beta-endorphin on initial estrogen action to induce lordosis behavior. Physiol Behav 1994, 55:157–62.
Gorzalka BB, Heddema GM, Lester GL, Hanson LA: beta-endorphin inhibits and facilitates lordosis behaviour in rats depending on ventricular site of administration. Neuropeptides 1997, 31:517–21.
Charbogne P, Kieffer BL, Befort K: 15 years of genetic approaches in vivo for addiction research: Opioid receptor and peptide gene knockout in mouse models of drug abuse. Neuropharmacology 2014, 76 Pt B:204–17.
Roth-Deri I, Green-Sadan T, Yadid G: Beta-endorphin and drug-induced reward and reinforcement. Prog Neurobiol 2008, 86:1–21.
Monteleone P: New frontiers in endocrinology of eating disorders. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 2011, 6:189–208.
Pecina S, Smith KS: Hedonic and motivational roles of opioids in food reward: implications for overeating disorders. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2010, 97:34–46.
Kringelbach ML, Berridge KC: The functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Discov Med 2010, 9:579–87.
Kringelbach ML, Berridge KC: The neuroscience of happiness and pleasure. Soc Res (New York) 2010, 77:659–78.
Berridge KC, Ho CY, Richard JM, DiFeliceantonio AG: The tempted brain eats: pleasure and desire circuits in obesity and eating disorders. Brain Res 2010, 1350:43–64.
Berridge KC: ‘Liking’ and ‘wanting’ food rewards: brain substrates and roles in eating disorders. Physiol Behav 2009, 97:537–50.
Benton D: The plausibility of sugar addiction and its role in obesity and eating disorders. Clin Nutr 2010, 29:288–303.
Fulton S: Appetite and reward. Front Neuroendocrinol 2010, 31:85–103.
Gosnell BA, Levine AS: Reward systems and food intake: role of opioids. Int J Obes (Lond) 2009,33(Suppl 2):S54–8.
Mahler SV, Berridge KC: Which cue to “want?” Central amygdala opioid activation enhances and focuses incentive salience on a prepotent reward cue. J Neurosci 2009, 29:6500–13.
Nathan PJ, Bullmore ET: From taste hedonics to motivational drive: central mu-opioid receptors and binge-eating behaviour. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2009, 12:995–1008.
Reece AS: Hypothalamic opioid-melanocortin appetitive balance and addictive craving. Med Hypotheses 2011, 76:132–7.
Yamamoto T: Central mechanisms of roles of taste in reward and eating. Acta Physiol Hung 2008, 95:165–86.
Cox LA, Nathanielsz PW: The importance of altered gene promoter methylation and transcription factor binding in developmental programming of central appetitive drive. J Physiol 2009, 587:4763–4.
Hasan TF, Hasan H: Anorexia nervosa: a unified neurological perspective. Int J Med Sci 2011, 8:679–703.
Bandelow B, Schmahl C, Falkai P, Wedekind D: Borderline personality disorder: a dysregulation of the endogenous opioid system? Psychol Rev 2010, 117:623–36.
Camacho FJ, Portillo W, Quintero-Enriquez O, Paredes RG: Reward value of intromissions and morphine in male rats evaluated by conditioned place preference. Physiol Behav 2009, 98:602–7.
Kippin TE, van der Kooy D: Excitotoxic lesions of the tegmental pedunculopontine nucleus impair copulation in naive male rats and block the rewarding effects of copulation in experienced male rats. Eur J Neurosci 2003, 18:2581–91.
Nocjar C, Panksepp J: Prior morphine experience induces long-term increases in social interest and in appetitive behavior for natural reward. Behav Brain Res 2007, 181:191–9.
Olivier JD, de Jong TR, Jos Dederen P, van Oorschot R, Heeren D, Pattij T, et al.: Effects of acute and chronic apomorphine on sex behavior and copulation-induced neural activation in the male rat. Eur J Pharmacol 2007, 576:61–76.
Paredes RG: Evaluating the neurobiology of sexual reward. ILAR J 2009, 50:15–27.
Pfaus JG: Pathways of sexual desire. J Sex Med 2009, 6:1506–33.
Pitchers KK, Frohmader KS, Vialou V, Mouzon E, Nestler EJ, Lehman MN, et al.: DeltaFosB in the nucleus accumbens is critical for reinforcing effects of sexual reward. Genes Brain Behav 2010, 9:831–40.
Riters LV: Evidence for opioid involvement in the motivation to sing. J Chem Neuroanat 2010, 39:141–50.
Tenk CM, Wilson H, Zhang Q, Pitchers KK, Coolen LM: Sexual reward in male rats: effects of sexual experience on conditioned place preferences associated with ejaculation and intromissions. Horm Behav 2009, 55:93–7.
Pitchers KK, Balfour ME, Lehman MN, Richtand NM, Yu L, Coolen LM: Neuroplasticity in the mesolimbic system induced by natural reward and subsequent reward abstinence. Biol Psychiatry 2010, 67:872–9.
Camacho FJ, Garcia-Horsman P, Paredes RG: Hormonal and testing conditions for the induction of conditioned place preference by paced mating. Horm Behav 2009, 56:410–5.
Nguyen AT, Marquez P, Hamid A, Kieffer B, Friedman TC, Lutfy K: The rewarding action of acute cocaine is reduced in beta-endorphin deficient but not in mu opioid receptor knockout mice. Eur J Pharmacol 2012, 686:50–4.
Simmons D, Self DW: Role of mu- and delta-opioid receptors in the nucleus accumbens in cocaine-seeking behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 2009, 34:1946–57.
Tseng A, Nguyen K, Hamid A, Garg M, Marquez P, Lutfy K: The role of endogenous beta-endorphin and enkephalins in ethanol reward. Neuropharmacology 2013, 73:290–300.
Zellner MR, Watt DF, Solms M, Panksepp J: Affective neuroscientific and neuropsychoanalytic approaches to two intractable psychiatric problems: why depression feels so bad and what addicts really want. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011, 35:2000–8.
Niikura K, Narita M, Butelman ER, Kreek MJ, Suzuki T: Neuropathic and chronic pain stimuli downregulate central mu-opioid and dopaminergic transmission. Trends Pharmacol Sci 2010, 31:299–305.
Trezza V, Damsteegt R, Achterberg EJ, Vanderschuren LJ: Nucleus accumbens mu-opioid receptors mediate social reward. J Neurosci 2011, 31:6362–70.
Nestler EJ: The neurobiology of cocaine addiction. Sci Pract Perspect 2005, 3:4–10.
Nestler EJ: Is there a common molecular pathway for addiction? Nat Neurosci 2005, 8:1445–9.
Di Chiara G, Imperato A: Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1988, 85:5274–8.
Spanagel R, Herz A, Shippenberg TS: Identification of the opioid receptor types mediating beta-endorphin-induced alterations in dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens. Eur J Pharmacol 1990, 190:177–84.
Spanagel R, Herz A, Shippenberg TS: The effects of opioid peptides on dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens: an in vivo microdialysis study. J Neurochem 1990, 55:1734–40.
Spanagel R, Weiss F: The dopamine hypothesis of reward: past and current status. Trends Neurosci 1999, 22:521–7.
Hadjiconstantinou M, Neff NH: Nicotine and endogenous opioids: neurochemical and pharmacological evidence. Neuropharmacology 2011, 60:1209–20.
Charmandari E, Tsigos C, Chrousos G: Endocrinology of the stress response. Annu Rev Physiol 2005, 67:259–84.
Sarnyai Z, Shaham Y, Heinrichs SC: The role of corticotropin-releasing factor in drug addiction. Pharmacol Rev 2001, 53:209–43.
Sauriyal DS, Jaggi AS, Singh N: Extending pharmacological spectrum of opioids beyond analgesia: multifunctional aspects in different pathophysiological states. Neuropeptides 2011, 45:175–88.
Tsigos C, Chrousos GP: Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, neuroendocrine factors and stress. J Psychosom Res 2002, 53:865–71.
Esch T, Stefano GB: The neurobiology of stress management. Neuro Endocrinol Lett 2010, 31:19–39.
Goeders NE, Guerin GF: Non-contingent electric footshock facilitates the acquisition of intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1994, 114:63–70.
Bilavsky E, Dagan A, Yarden-Bilavsky H, Davidovits M, Shapiro R, Mor E, et al.: Adrenal insufficiency during physiological stress in children after kidney or liver transplantation. Pediatr Transplant 2011, 15:314–20.
Green-Sadan T, Kinor N, Roth-Deri I, Geffen-Aricha R, Schindler CJ, Yadid G: Transplantation of glial cell line-derived neurotrophic factor-expressing cells into the striatum and nucleus accumbens attenuates acquisition of cocaine self-administration in rats. Eur J Neurosci 2003, 18:2093–8.
Hadjiconstantinou M, Duchemin AM, Zhang H, Neff NH: Enhanced dopamine transporter function in striatum during nicotine withdrawal. Synapse 2011, 65:91–8.
Shi J, Li SX, Zhang XL, Wang X, Le Foll B, Zhang XY, et al.: Time-dependent neuroendocrine alterations and drug craving during the first month of abstinence in heroin addicts. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 2009, 35:267–72.
Bachtell RK, Self DW: Effects of adenosine A2A receptor stimulation on cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009, 206:469–78.
Ahmed SH, Koob GF: Transition to drug addiction: a negative reinforcement model based on an allostatic decrease in reward function. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005, 180:473–90.
Barfield ET, Barry SM, Hodgin HB, Thompson BM, Allen SS, Grisel JE: Beta-endorphin mediates behavioral despair and the effect of ethanol on the tail suspension test in mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2010, 34:1066–72.
Sapolsky RM: Why zebra's don’t get ulcers. 3rd edition. New York: Henry Holt & Cy; 2004.
Swanson LW, Sawchenko PE: Hypothalamic integration: organization of the paraventricular and supraoptic nuclei. Annu Rev Neurosci 1983, 6:269–324.
Swanson LW, Sawchenko PE, Lind RW, Rho JH: The CRH motoneuron: differential peptide regulation in neurons with possible synaptic, paracrine, and endocrine outputs. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1987, 512:12–23.
Swanson LW: The hypothalamus. In Integrated systems of the CNS, part 1. Edited by: Björklund A, Hökfelt T, Swanson LW. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1987:1–124. Handbook of chemical neuroanatomy
Veening JG, van der Meer MJ, Joosten H, Hermus AR, Rijnnkels CE, Geeraedts LM, et al.: Intravenous administration of interleukin-1 beta induces Fos-like immunoreactivity in corticotropin-releasing hormone neurons in the paraventricular hypothalamic nucleus of the rat. J Chem Neuroanat 1993, 6:391–7.
Veening JG, Coolen LM, Spooren WJ, Joosten H, van Oorschot R, Mos J, et al.: Patterns of c-fos expression induced by fluvoxamine are different after acute vs. chronic oral administration. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 1998, 8:213–26.
Veening JG, Bouwknecht JA, Joosten HJ, Dederen PJ, Zethof TJ, Groenink L, et al.: Stress-induced hyperthermia in the mouse: c-fos expression, corticosterone and temperature changes. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2004, 28:699–707.
Groenink L, Dirks A, Verdouw PM, Schipholt M, Veening JG, van der Gugten J, et al.: HPA axis dysregulation in mice overexpressing corticotropin releasing hormone. Biol Psychiatry 2002, 51:875–81.
Veening JG, Bocker KB, Verdouw PM, Olivier B, De Jongh R, Groenink L: Activation of the septohippocampal system differentiates anxiety from fear in startle paradigms. Neuroscience 2009, 163:1046–60.
Grisel JE, Bartels JL, Allen SA, Turgeon VL: Influence of beta-Endorphin on anxious behavior in mice: interaction with EtOH. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008, 200:105–15.
Bergasa NV, Rothman RB, Mukerjee E, Vergalla J, Jones EA: Up-regulation of central mu-opioid receptors in a model of hepatic encephalopathy: a potential mechanism for increased sensitivity to morphine in liver failure. Life Sci 2002, 70:1701–8.
Quang PN, Schmidt BL: Peripheral endothelin B receptor agonist-induced antinociception involves endogenous opioids in mice. Pain 2010, 149:254–262.
Hagerty MR, Isaacs J, Brasington L, Shupe L, Fetz EE, Cramer SC: Case study of ecstatic meditation: fMRI and EEG evidence of self-stimulating a reward system. Neural Plast 2013, 2013:653572.
Harte JL, Eifert GH: The effects of running, environment, and attentional focus on athletes’ catecholamine and cortisol levels and mood. Psychophysiology 1995, 32:49–54.
Harte JL, Eifert GH, Smith R: The effects of running and meditation on beta-endorphin, corticotropin-releasing hormone and cortisol in plasma, and on mood. Biol Psychol 1995, 40:251–65.
Infante JR, Peran F, Martinez M, Roldan A, Poyatos R, Ruiz C, et al.: ACTH and beta-endorphin in transcendental meditation. Physiol Behav 1998, 64:311–5.
Koob GF, Le Moal M: Plasticity of reward neurocircuitry and the ‘dark side’ of drug addiction. Nat Neurosci 2005, 8:1442–4.
Papathanassoglou ED, Giannakopoulou M, Mpouzika M, Bozas E, Karabinis A: Potential effects of stress in critical illness through the role of stress neuropeptides. Nurs Crit Care 2010, 15:204–16.
Roth-Deri I, Zangen A, Aleli M, Goelman RG, Pelled G, Nakash R, et al.: Effect of experimenter-delivered and self-administered cocaine on extracellular beta-endorphin levels in the nucleus accumbens. J Neurochem 2003, 84:930–8.
Veehof MM, Oskam MJ, Schreurs KM, Bohlmeijer ET: Acceptance-based interventions for the treatment of chronic pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pain 2011, 152:533–42.
Yadav RK, Magan D, Mehta M, Mehta N, Mahapatra SC: A short-term, comprehensive, yoga-based lifestyle intervention is efficacious in reducing anxiety, improving subjective well-being and personality. Int J Yoga 2012, 5:134–9.
Slominski A, Wortsman J, Luger T, Paus R, Solomon S: Corticotropin releasing hormone and proopiomelanocortin involvement in the cutaneous response to stress. Physiol Rev 2000, 80:979–1020.
Tsigos C, Crosby SR, Gibson S, Young RJ, White A: Proopiomelanocortin is the predominant adrenocorticotropin-related peptide in human cerebrospinal fluid. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1993, 76:620–4.
de Kloet ER: Stress: a neurobiological perspective. Tijdschr Psychiatr 2009, 51:541–50.
de Kloet ER, Fitzsimons CP, Datson NA, Meijer OC, Vreugdenhil E: Glucocorticoid signaling and stress-related limbic susceptibility pathway: about receptors, transcription machinery and microRNA. Brain Res 2009, 1293:129–41.
Yadav RK, Magan D, Mehta N, Sharma R, Mahapatra SC: Efficacy of a short-term yoga-based lifestyle intervention in reducing stress and inflammation: preliminary results. J Altern Complement Med 2012, 18:662–7.
de Kloet ER: From vasotocin to stress and cognition. Eur J Pharmacol 2010, 626:18–26.
Shapiro SL, Brown KW, Thoresen C, Plante TG: The moderation of Mindfulness-based stress reduction effects by trait mindfulness: results from a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Psychol 2011, 67:267–77.
Dobkin PL, Zhao Q: Increased mindfulness–the active component of the mindfulness-based stress reduction program? Complement Ther Clin Pract 2011, 17:22–7.
Schmidt S, Grossman P, Schwarzer B, Jena S, Naumann J, Walach H: Treating fibromyalgia with mindfulness-based stress reduction: results from a 3-armed randomized controlled trial. Pain 2011, 152:361–9.
Esmer G, Blum J, Rulf J, Pier J: Mindfulness-based stress reduction for failed back surgery syndrome: a randomized controlled trial. J Am Osteopath Assoc 2010, 110:646–52.
Merkes M: Mindfulness-based stress reduction for people with chronic diseases. Aust J Prim Health 2010, 16:200–10.
Goldin PR, Gross JJ: Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder. Emotion 2010, 10:83–91.
Perlman DM, Salomons TV, Davidson RJ, Lutz A: Differential effects on pain intensity and unpleasantness of two meditation practices. Emotion 2010, 10:65–71.
Rosenzweig S, Greeson JM, Reibel DK, Green JS, Jasser SA, Beasley D: Mindfulness-based stress reduction for chronic pain conditions: variation in treatment outcomes and role of home meditation practice. J Psychosom Res 2010, 68:29–36.
Chiesa A: Vipassana meditation: systematic review of current evidence. J Altern Complement Med 2010, 16:37–46.
Chiesa A, Calati R, Serretti A: Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. Clin Psychol Rev 2011, 31:449–64.
Chiesa A, Serretti A: Mindfulness based cognitive therapy for psychiatric disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res 2011, 187:441–53.
Zeidan F, Martucci KT, Kraft RA, Gordon NS, McHaffie JG, Coghill RC: Brain mechanisms supporting the modulation of pain by mindfulness meditation. J Neurosci 2011, 31:5540–8.
Abbott RA, Whear R, Rodgers LR, Bethel A, Thompson Coon J, Kuyken W, et al.: Effectiveness of mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness based cognitive therapy in vascular disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. J Psychosom Res 2014, 76:341–51.
Black DS: Mindfulness-based interventions: an antidote to suffering in the context of substance use, misuse, and addiction. Subst Use Misuse 2014, 49:487–91.
Eisendrath SJ, Gillung EP, Delucchi KL, Chartier M, Mathalon DH, Sullivan JC, et al.: Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) versus the health-enhancement program (HEP) for adults with treatment-resistant depression: a randomized control trial study protocol. BMC Complement Altern Med 2014, 14:95.
Kopf S, Oikonomou D, Hartmann M, Feier F, Faude-Lang V, Morcos M, et al.: Effects of stress reduction on cardiovascular risk factors in type 2 diabetes patients with early kidney disease - results of a randomized controlled trial (HEIDIS). Exp Clin Endocrinol Diabetes 2014,122(6):341–9. doi: 10.1055/s-0034–1372583. Epub 2014 May 5. PubMed PMID: 24798861
Lenze EJ, Hickman S, Hershey T, Wendleton L, Ly K, Dixon D, et al.: Mindfulness-based stress reduction for older adults with worry symptoms and co-occurring cognitive dysfunction. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2014,29(10):991–1000. doi: 10.1002/gps.4086. Epub 2014 Feb 18. PubMed PMID: 24677282; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4136987
Smith SA: Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction: An Intervention to Enhance the Effectiveness of Nurses' Coping With Work-Related Stress. Int J Nurs Knowl 2014,25(2):119–30. doi: 10.1111/2047–3095.12025. Epub 2014 Feb 26. PubMed PMID: 24612607
Rapgay L, Bystrisky A: Classical mindfulness: an introduction to its theory and practice for clinical application. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009, 1172:148–62.
Chiesa A, Serretti A: A systematic review of neurobiological and clinical features of mindfulness meditations. Psychol Med 2010, 40:1239–52.
Moynihan JA, Chapman BP, Klorman R, Krasner MS, Duberstein PR, Brown KW, et al.: Mindfulness-based stress reduction for older adults: effects on executive function, frontal alpha asymmetry and immune function. Neuropsychobiology 2013, 68:34–43.
Pickut BA, Van Hecke W, Kerckhofs E, Marien P, Vanneste S, Cras P, et al.: Mindfulness based intervention in Parkinson’s disease leads to structural brain changes on MRI: a randomized controlled longitudinal trial. Clin Neurol Neurosurg 2013, 115:2419–25.
Wells RE, Yeh GY, Kerr CE, Wolkin J, Davis RB, Tan Y, et al.: Meditation’s impact on default mode network and hippocampus in mild cognitive impairment: a pilot study. Neurosci Lett 2013, 556:15–9.
Holzel BK, Carmody J, Vangel M, Congleton C, Yerramsetti SM, Gard T, et al.: Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Res 2011, 191:36–43.
Holzel BK, Carmody J, Evans KC, Hoge EA, Dusek JA, Morgan L, et al.: Stress reduction correlates with structural changes in the amygdala. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2010, 5:11–7.
Jovanovic H, Perski A, Berglund H, Savic I: Chronic stress is linked to 5-HT(1A) receptor changes and functional disintegration of the limbic networks. Neuroimage 2011, 55:1178–88.
Kerr CE, Sacchet MD, Lazar SW, Moore CI, Jones SR: Mindfulness starts with the body: somatosensory attention and top-down modulation of cortical alpha rhythms in mindfulness meditation. Front Hum Neurosci 2013, 7:12.
Goldin P, Ziv M, Jazaieri H, Hahn K, Gross JJ: MBSR vs aerobic exercise in social anxiety: fMRI of emotion regulation of negative self-beliefs. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013, 8:65–72.
Hegadoren KM, O’Donnell T, Lanius R, Coupland NJ, Lacaze-Masmonteil N: The role of beta-endorphin in the pathophysiology of major depression. Neuropeptides 2009, 43:341–53.
Panksepp J: Affective neuroscience of the emotional BrainMind: evolutionary perspectives and implications for understanding depression. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 2010, 12:533–45.
Coenen VA, Schlaepfer TE, Maedler B, Panksepp J: Cross-species affective functions of the medial forebrain bundle-implications for the treatment of affective pain and depression in humans. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011, 35:1971–81.
Harro J, Kanarik M, Matrov D, Panksepp J: Mapping patterns of depression-related brain regions with cytochrome oxidase histochemistry: relevance of animal affective systems to human disorders, with a focus on resilience to adverse events. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011, 35:1876–89.
Bernroider G, Panksepp J: Mirrors and feelings: have you seen the actors outside? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011, 35:2009–16.
Panksepp J: Toward a cross-species neuroscientific understanding of the affective mind: do animals have emotional feelings? Am J Primatol 2011, 73:545–61.
Stein DJ, van Honk J, Ipser J, Solms M, Panksepp J: Opioids: from physical pain to the pain of social isolation. CNS Spectr 2007, 12:669–70.
Panksepp J, Herman BH, Vilberg T, Bishop P, DeEskinazi FG: Endogenous opioids and social behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1980, 4:473–87.
Akil H, Richardson DE, Barchas JD, Li CH: Appearance of beta-endorphin-like immunoreactivity in human ventricular cerebrospinal fluid upon analgesic electrical stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1978, 75:5170–2.
Akil H, Richardson DE, Hughes J, Barchas JD: Enkephalin-like material elevated in ventricular cerebrospinal fluid of pain patients after analgetic focal stimulation. Science 1978, 201:463–5.
Guillemin R, Ling N, Burgus R, Bloom F, Segal D: Characterization of the endorphins, novel hypothalamic and neurohypophysial peptides with opiate-like activity: evidence that they induce profound behavioral changes. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1977, 2:59–62.
Guillemin R, Ling N, Lazarus L, Burgus R, Minick S, Bloom F, et al.: The endorphins, novel peptides of brain and hypophysial origin, with opiate-like activity: biochemical and biologic studies. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1977, 297:131–57.
Segal DS, Browne RG, Bloom F, Ling N, Guillemin R: beta-Endorphin: endogenous opiate or neuroleptic? Science 1977, 198:411–4.
Bloom F, Battenberg E, Rossier J, Ling N, Leppaluoto J, Vargo TM, et al.: Endorphins are located in the intermediate and anterior lobes of the pituitary gland, not in the neurohypophysis. Life Sci 1977, 20:43–7.
Bloom F, Segal D, Ling N, Guillemin R: Endorphins: profound behavioral effects in rats suggest new etiological factors in mental illness. Science 1976, 194:630–2.
Frederickson RC, Geary LE: Endogenous opioid peptides: review of physiological, pharmacological and clinical aspects. Prog Neurobiol 1982, 19:19–69.
O’Donohue TL, Dorsa DM: The opiomelanotropinergic neuronal and endocrine systems. Peptides 1982, 3:353–95.
Loh HH, Tseng LF, Wei E, Li CH: beta-endorphin is a potent analgesic agent. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1976, 73:2895–8.
Foley KM, Kourides IA, Inturrisi CE, Kaiko RF, Zaroulis CG, Posner JB, et al.: beta-Endorphin: analgesic and hormonal effects in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1979, 76:5377–81.
Backryd E, Ghafouri B, Larsson B, Gerdle B: Do low levels of beta-endorphin in the cerebrospinal fluid indicate defective top-down inhibition in patients with chronic neuropathic pain? A cross-sectional, comparative study. Pain Med 2014, 15:111–9.
Feldreich A, Ernberg M, Lund B, Rosen A: Increased beta-endorphin levels and generalized decreased pain thresholds in patients with limited jaw opening and movement-evoked pain from t